Skip to main content

Reproductive stoppage in autism spectrum disorder in a population of 2.5 million individuals

Abstract

Background

It has been suggested that parents of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) curtail their reproduction, a phenomenon known as reproductive stoppage. To investigate the presence of reproductive stoppage, we followed the reproduction in mothers of children with or without an ASD diagnosis using Swedish population-based registries.

Methods

We followed all families with first child born in 1987 or later. In total 2,521,103 children, nested within 1,270,017 mothers, were included. Exposure was presence of ASD diagnosis in earlier born siblings, and outcome was considered as (1) inter-pregnancy interval and (2) number of subsequent children.

Results

Analyses of inter-pregnancy intervals showed that the association differed across birth orders, with a lower rate of second children when first child had ASD diagnosis, but an increased rate of third and higher birth orders in families where a previous child had an ASD diagnosis. When all birth orders were simultaneously considered, families with a child with an ASD diagnosis were less likely to have another child (hazard ratio (HR), 0.79; 95% confidence interval [95% CI], 0.78–0.80). However, when adjusted for birth order, the association was close to null (HR, 0.97; 95% CI, 0.96–0.99), and after additional adjustments (maternal age, birth period, sex, paternal age, and maternal education), the association disappeared (HR, 1.00; 95% CI, 0.99–1.02). In analyses of subsequent children, after adjustment for covariates, families with an ASD diagnosis had 4% more subsequent children (rate ratio, 1.04; 95% CI, 1.03–1.05).

Limitations

The study was undertaken in a country with largely tax-funded healthcare; results may not generalize to other societies. Following the current dominating umbrella concept of ASD, we did not differentiate between the ASD sub-diagnoses; it is possible that reproductive patterns can be dependent on ASD subtypes and the severity and composition of ASD phenotypes and comorbidities.

Conclusions

This study does not support a universal reproductive stoppage effect in ASD families, when birth order and other factors are considered. Therefore, proper attention to birth order and other factors may alleviate potential bias in familial aggregation studies of ASD.

Background

It has been suggested that parents who have a child with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may curtail their reproduction compared to parents who do not have a child with these impairments. This phenomenon is often referred to as reproductive stoppage [1,2,3,4]. The definition of reproductive stoppage varies and has in recent studies been operationalized as a change in inter-pregnancy interval (time to next child) depending on whether a prior born child has an ASD diagnosis [3, 4]. However, a change in total number of subsequent children is equally compatible with the concept, as suggested in a recent study by Wood et al., who define reproductive stoppage as “following the diagnosis of ASD in a family, parents may change plans regarding their family size” [5].

Stoppage is often mentioned as a potential source of bias in family studies of ASD [3, 4, 6,7,8,9]. Recent research found evidence of reproductive stoppage when estimating inter-pregnancy intervals or number of second-born children in parents whose first-born had ASD [3, 4]. Still, to the best of our knowledge, neither an overall effect on inter-pregnancy intervals—estimating a combined effect over different birth orders—nor the effect on number of subsequent children, as an estimate of stoppage on total family size, has been investigated in a population-based cohort. It therefore remains unknown whether evidence of reproductive stoppage remains if (i) combined over birth orders and if (ii) it is present when assessing number of subsequent children. Considering its potential to introduce bias in studies on ASD, it is of great importance to scrutinize the potential stoppage effect, not in specific subsamples but in the population as a whole.

The aim of this study was to examine reproductive stoppage in a total population followed up longitudinally, including all births in Sweden between 1987 and 2013. We examined the effect of having a child with ASD diagnosis on mother’s reproduction. More specifically, compared to families without children with ASD diagnosis, we investigated both the inter-pregnancy intervals and the number of subsequent children in families having:

  1. 1.

    Any earlier born children with an ASD diagnosis using all birth orders combined,

  2. 2.

    Immediately previous born child with an ASD diagnosis for all birth orders combined,

  3. 3.

    Different combinations of prior born children with ASD diagnoses.

Methods

Study cohort

We identified the study cohort by linking population-wide registers in Sweden. From the Total Population Register [10], we identified all individuals born between January 1, 1987, and December 31, 2013, (N = 3,268,205). The Multi-Generation Register [11] was used to identify parents and birth order by mother, excluding individuals without identifiable parents (133,253 excluded). We included only births of mothers who had their first child in 1987 or later (381,349 excluded). We excluded families with multiple births (e.g., twins; 113,552 excluded) and only included the first five live born children by mother in each family (10,893 excluded), ignoring whether the siblings were full or half. Mothers in these families were followed until death, date of first emigration, or end of follow-up (December 31, 2013), whichever came first. Children born after the mothers were censored due to emigration out of Sweden were excluded (108,055 excluded) resulting in a dataset consisting of 2,521,103 births nested within 1,270,017 mothers. For all analyses, we further limited the analytic dataset to births prior January 1, 2012 (to ensure a possibility of at least 2 years of follow-up), resulting in 2,296,137 inter-pregnancy intervals. Of these, 1,246,227 ended in another birth and 1,049,910 were censored.

This study was conducted on anonymized data and the study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm, Sweden.

Exposures

We linked the study cohort dataset to the National Patient Register, which gathers diagnoses from inpatient healthcare throughout the follow-up, and from outpatient visits to specialist care from 2001 and onwards [12], and identified ASD diagnoses in the children according to International Classification of Diseases, 9th revision, (ICD-9; years 1987–1996, code 299) and ICD, 10th version (ICD-10; years 1997–2013, codes F84.0, F84.1, F84.5, F84.8, F84.9). Similar to an earlier study [3], we did not consider the timing of diagnosis meaningful and regarded children as having ASD from early age regardless of when the first diagnosis was registered. We did this because age of diagnosis is dependent on several factors, such as availability of services and the diagnostic routines and preferences of the time, which has changed between 1987 and 2013. Further, the severity of ASD also affects the age when caregivers seek help and diagnostic assessment is conducted. DSM-5 criteria are consistent with this reasoning, stating that symptoms of ASD must be present in childhood but might not necessarily be fully manifested until later life, when social demands exceed the individuals’ capabilities [13].

To ensure a complete picture of potential stoppage effects, we defined being exposed to ASD in families in several ways:

  1. 1.

    Any before—Any previously born child by the same mother had an ASD diagnosis.

  2. 2.

    Immediately before—The immediately previously born child having ASD diagnosis.

  3. 3.

    Birth order specific—For first- to second-born children we compared families where the first-born had ASD diagnosis to families where the first-born was not diagnosed with ASD. For second- to third-born, we calculated exposure as (a) only first-born having ASD diagnosis, (b) only second-born having ASD diagnosis, and (c) both first- and second-born having ASD diagnosis and compared to families without children with ASD diagnosis.

Outcomes

Inter-pregnancy intervals

We calculated the time from a birth until next birth, or censoring, for births up until fifth child of a mother.

Number of subsequent children

We counted the number of children born to the same mother after each birth. To account for differing time of follow-up, we calculated time remaining until censored or age 55 for mothers for each first to fourth births. The choice of 55 as the upper limit was guided by the data, since almost no births were observed after this age.

Covariates

As covariates (all categorical), we firstly included birth order, a confounder since the more siblings in a family the more likely that at least one of them has ASD, and if parents plan their family size, birth order will also affect the outcome. Secondly, we included birth period (1987–1991, 1992–1996, 1997–2001, 2002–2006, 2007–2011) to alleviate spurious associations due to changes in rates of ASD diagnoses (steeply increasing over time in current sample) and changes in average family sizes over time. Thirdly, maternal and paternal age were investigated, since they are associated with probability of ASD in offspring [14] and with likelihood of having more children. Fourthly, we included highest maternal education, gathered from the Longitudinal Database for Health Insurance and Labor Market Studies [15], as a proxy of socioeconomic status, as it may be associated with both risk for ASD and family size. Finally, we considered sex of offspring in index birth, since the risk for ASD differs between the sexes and that the sex composition of the siblings may affect parents’ decision to have another child.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive

For children with and without an ASD diagnosis, we summarized the data on individual level (for covariates sex, birth order, maternal and paternal age), family level (number of children, follow-up time, rate of childbearing, family size, maternal education level), and inter-pregnancy interval level (observed and number censored intervals, interval length). We calculated inverse Kaplan-Meier curves (i.e., 1 minus the survival function) for the cumulative proportion having another child according to the three exposure definitions.

Inter-pregnancy intervals

We performed survival analyses using Cox proportional hazards regression, where event was defined as “having another child,” to analyze the potential stoppage effect in terms of delaying next child and/or not having another child. We analyzed the data for the different exposure definitions: crude, adjusted for one covariate at time, and then adjusted for all covariates. For analyses with exposure definitions (1) and (2), i.e., where birth orders were combined, all included children of a mother were considered, and adjustment for birth order was performed by stratified Cox regression to allow differing baseline hazards.

Number of subsequent children

We used Poisson regression to analyze the childbearing rate after an index childbirth to examine the potential limiting of planned family size. The number of subsequent children was considered the outcome; the time left that a mother possibly could have children (i.e., until censored or 55 years of age) was included as an offset term in the analyses. We also split the follow-up time into different maternal age periods (according to levels in Table 1) and counted the number of children born within each period to allow different baseline rates of childbearing within different maternal ages. We analyzed the data for the different exposure definitions: crude, adjusted for one covariate at time, and then adjusted for all covariates.

Table 1 Descriptive information on the first four children born in analyzed families, on individual-, family-, and inter-pregnancy interval levels

Sensitivity analysis

Birth order, third- to fourth-born

For third- to fourth-born inter-pregnancy interval, we calculated inverse Kaplan-Meier curves where we compared families without children with ASD diagnosis with families where (a) only first-born had ASD, (b) only second-born had ASD, (c) only third-born had ASD, and (d) at least two of first- to third-born having ASD.

Birth cohort specific associations

To investigate potential bias arising from differing length of follow-up—for instance, bias due to too short follow-up to ascertain diagnosis in the offspring—we performed analyses for inter-pregnancy intervals stratified on birth year of first-born (in same categories as listed above).

All analyses were performed using R base functions [16] and library “survival” [17]. Precision of estimates are presented as 95% confidence intervals, adjusted using the sandwich estimator, to account for deviations from basic modeling assumptions (such as homoscedasticity), from the “sandwich” library [18].

We tested the proportional hazards assumption for the most inclusive “Any before”-exposure definition. The test showed an acceptable low deviation from proportionality; the correlation between the Schoenfeld residuals and Kaplan-Meier-transformed time was − 0.002 with a p value of 0.012 (an acceptable p value given the large sample [19]).

Role of the funding source

The funders had no role in the design of the study; collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; in writing the report; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Results

Descriptive

In Table 1, descriptive information on the population is presented. Our analyzed cohort, the first four children born to a mother, comprises a total of 1,270,017 families with 2,503,434 children with a total of 26,842 (1.1%) children with ASD diagnosis. More males than females were diagnosed with ASD, and diagnosed children were more likely to be first-born. Moreover, children with ASD were generally born to younger parents. Families with children with ASD diagnosis had, on average, more children than those without (2.4 vs 2.0), and a longer follow-up (17.6 vs 13.0 years), but a lower mean rate of childbearing (137.7 vs 153.6 children per 1000 years). Further, mothers in families with children with ASD diagnosis had lower education level than mothers in families without. For inter-pregnancy intervals, the number of intervals ending with a birth (rather than being censored) was higher in families with children with ASD diagnosis (60.2%) than in families without (54.2%). This was also observed by birth order, except for first- to second-born where families without first-born with ASD diagnosis showed a higher rate of observed second births. Families with children with ASD diagnosis generally had longer inter-pregnancy intervals between two consecutive births, as well as from a birth until censoring.

Figure 1a show a pattern indicating a potential stoppage effect, collapsed over all childbirths; families with prior born children with ASD diagnosis had less subsequent children. In Fig. 1b, the same graph is presented but considering if the immediately prior born child has ASD, whereafter the pattern supportive for stoppage completely disappears. Further, while families with children with ASD diagnosis show a lower likelihood of getting a second child if the first-born has ASD diagnosis (Fig. 1c), families with two children show a higher likelihood to get a third child if any of the two have an ASD diagnosis (Fig. 1d).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Proportion having another child if any of the previous children received an ASD diagnosis. All inter-pregnancy intervals included. Estimates, 95% confidence intervals, and numbers at risk. b Proportion having another child if the immediately prior born child received an ASD diagnosis. All inter-pregnancy intervals included. Estimates, 95% confidence intervals, and numbers at risk. c Proportion having a second child if the first-born child receives an ASD diagnosis. Estimates, 95% confidence intervals, and numbers at risk. d Proportion having a third child if the first- and second-born children received ASD diagnoses. Estimates and numbers at risk (note: different scale on y-axis; in Additional file 1: Figure S1, this plot is presented with 95% confidence intervals)

Inter-pregnancy intervals

In Table 2, all Cox analyses for inter-pregnancy intervals are presented. The crude HRs, using no covariates, reflected in Fig. 1, suggested that mothers of ASD diagnosed children waited longer until having another child and/or were less likely to have another child compared to mothers of children without ASD (hazard ratio [HR], 0.79; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.78–0.80). Among the considered covariates, birth order attenuated this effect most (HR, 0.97; 95% CI, 0.96–0.98). When all covariates were adjusted for, the association was non-existing (HR, 1.00; 95% CI, 0.99–1.02), indicating that the result was due to differences in considered covariates between families with or without children with ASD diagnosis. The HRs were above unity when only immediately previous child was considered as exposure (HR, 1.05; 95% CI, 1.03–1.07) but changed to below unity after covariate adjustment (HR, 0.97; 95% CI, 0.95–0.98).

Table 2 Analyses of inter-pregnancy intervals, Cox proportional hazards regression of time from one birth to next

For first to second pregnancy interval, the unadjusted HR was 0.87 (95% CI, 0.86–0.89) and remained lower than unity after covariate adjustment (HR, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.88–0.91). Conversely, for second to third pregnancy intervals, the HRs were well above unity and remained so after covariate adjustment (HR range 1.05 to 1.17), suggesting that families with ASD diagnosed children had shorter intervals between children and/or were more likely to have another child.

Number of subsequent children

In Table 3, all Poisson regression analyses of childbearing rates are presented. As observed in the descriptive data, the crude rate of childbearing in families with any previous child having an ASD diagnosis was lower than in families without children with ASD diagnosis, rate ratio (RR), 0.85 (95% CI, 0.84–0.86), meaning that families with children with ASD diagnosis had 15% less subsequent children. Adjustment for birth order had the largest effect on this estimate (RR, 0.97; 95% CI, 0.96–0.98). The effect was reversed when all covariates were adjusted for and when baseline birth rates were modeled to be different for different maternal ages (RR, 1.04; 95% CI, 1.03–1.05). The results were similar for the immediately previous born having ASD, but of smaller magnitude.

Table 3 Analysis of number of subsequent children, Poisson regression analysis of rates as number of children per follow-up time

When analyzing numbers of subsequent children after first-born, comparing ASD diagnosed with undiagnosed, we found a limiting effect both in the crude (RR, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.89–0.92) and fully adjusted model (RR, 0.98; 95% CI, 0.96–0.99), similarly to the analysis of inter-pregnancy intervals. By contrast, the analysis of number of subsequent children after second-born showed that families with ASD diagnosed children had higher rates of childbearing, with and without covariate adjustments (RR range 1.06 to 1.15).

Sensitivity analysis

Birth order, third- to fourth-born

The inverse Kaplan-Meier curves for third- to fourth-born indicated higher number of children in families with children with ASD diagnosis (Additional file 1: Figures S2 and S3).

Birth cohort specific associations

When analyzing inter-pregnancy intervals in families stratified on the first-born child’s birth years, the result remained similar (Additional file 1: Table S1). The birth order specific estimates had CIs that all contained the estimate from the main analyses. In all birth periods, the result from combined birth orders had CIs containing 1 or the full CI was above 1, except the last birth period (with shortest follow-up) with HR and CI below 1. However, potential problems with bias due to lack of information on diagnosis should be largest in the birth cohort with shortest follow-up.

Discussion

This study investigated the hypothesis that parents having a child diagnosed with ASD change their reproductive behaviors towards having fewer subsequent children—commonly referred to as reproductive stoppage. We used two variables capturing stoppage effects, inter-pregnancy interval (time to next child), and number of subsequent children in a population sample of 2,521,103 children in 1,270,017 families. Our results did not support the hypothesis of a universal stoppage effect in families with children with ASD diagnosis, but results depended on birth orders. The group of parents whose first-born had an ASD diagnosis tended to be less likely to have another child and had smaller families. However, among those who did have a second child, and among those who had a child with ASD diagnosis of later birth order, the overall family size was larger than in families without children with ASD diagnosis. Notably, when analyzing these effects combined over birth order (and other covariates), they tended to balance out for inter-pregnancy intervals (Table 2) and to be in the direction of more children in families with children with ASD diagnosis for number of subsequent children (Table 3).

Although our results did not confirm previous research regarding universal stoppage [3, 4], it concurs with a Danish study for first to second birth [4]. They, however, did not consider a combined effect over birth orders; therefore, we cannot compare the combined effects directly between the cohorts. Furthermore, the results from a Californian study [3] differ in magnitude of estimates from ours and in direction/significance of estimates in adjusted analyses. Both previous large studies only analyzed time-to-next-child; we are thus unable to compare our results regarding number of subsequent children directly.

The observed differences may be due to societal differences between Scandinavian countries and California. Healthcare in Sweden is largely tax-funded, a system that ensures everyone has equal access to healthcare services, as well as to other welfare and support systems (e.g., parental leave). In the USA, families with children with ASD diagnosis might suffer financially more than in Sweden. This might explain why a stoppage effect is present in the Californian study but not in the present study. Further studies are needed in order to investigate if the healthcare system affects reproductive behavior. Alternatively, the difference could be due to ascertainment differences; the present study is based on population-based health registers, minimizing the risk of selection bias, while the Californian study was based on a non-random sample from a target population rather than total population, which may explain some of the difference in results.

The experience of having a child with ASD may differ across families. Some parents may experience more psychological strain of rearing a child with ASD and therefore choose to not have subsequent children (i.e., the lower rate of second births in these families). Relatedly, the ASD phenotype is highly heterogeneous with some individuals requiring almost constant support from early childhood, while, for others, the ASD symptomatology does not manifest itself until late childhood, and then often in milder forms. Thus, the severity of the ASD phenotype may affect the choice to conceive another child. Additionally, parental characteristics expressed when becoming a parent and in the parent-child relationship vary and, therefore, the parents’ psychological perception of having a child with ASD may differ. Hence, it could be that it is not the ASD phenotype per se that causes the observed associations but, rather, the parental experience. Possibly, these two effects interact with regard to parental decision on future childbearing. Thus, future research may investigate the psychological ramifications and potential differences of experience for parents of having a child with ASD diagnosis.

Finally, our results indicate that, in family studies of ASD, proper attention to covariates, particularly birth order, may be sufficient to alleviate potential biases due to differences in childbearing in families with children with ASD diagnosis compared to those without, at least when familial effects are not considered to be birth order specific.

Limitations

The results of this study needs to be viewed in light of certain limitations. We based our ASD definition on the National Patient Register, where we are likely to face an under-detection of true cases in the population, since not all cases are diagnosed in clinical practice and reported to the register. However, the under-reporting is likely to be small since ASD is today apparently rather consequently diagnosed in Sweden, as indicated by relatively high national prevalence rates from a comparative international view [20, 21], and the majority of individuals with ASD will receive care by specialist care who report to the National Patient Register. We assumed ASD present prior to it being observed in the registries, which potentially introduce bias. However, a sensitivity analysis based on families with different birth years of first-born child yielded results which did not deviate notably from the overall results. Finally, following the current dominating umbrella concept of ASD, we did not differentiate between the included ASD sub-diagnoses; it is possible that reproductive patterns can be dependent on ASD subtypes and the severity and composition of ASD phenotypes and comorbidities.

The main strengths of this study are its large sample of a non-selected population and the prospectively collected data, as well as the several ways the exposure and outcome were conceptualized. Importantly, Sweden has equal health access for its inhabitants making bias due to systematic differences in likelihood of being diagnosed unlikely. Further, analyses of an entire cohort with prospectively collected data, as in the present study, minimizes risk of non-representability of analyzed sample in terms of comparisons of families with and without children with ASD diagnosis.

Conclusion

This population-based cohort study of more than 2,500,000 individuals does not support the hypothesis of a universal reproductive stoppage effect in ASD families, when birth order and other factors are considered. Proper attention to birth order may alleviate potential bias in familial aggregation studies of ASD.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to Swedish Law, but are available from the register holders (Statistics Sweden; Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare) on request, after ethical vetting by an Ethical Review Board.

Abbreviations

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

CI:

Confidence interval

HR:

Hazard ratio

RR:

Rate ratio

References

  1. Jones MB, Szatmari P. Stoppage rules and genetic studies of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 1988;18:31–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Slager S, Foroud T, Haghighi F, Spence M, Hodge S. Stoppage: an issue for segregation analysis. Genet Epidemiol. 2001;20:328–39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Hoffmann TJ, Windham GC, Anderson M, Croen LA, Grether JK, Risch N. Evidence of reproductive stoppage in families with autism spectrum disorder: a large, population-based cohort study. JAMA psychiatry. 2014;71:943–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Grønborg TK, Hansen SN, Nielsen SV, Skytthe A, Parner ET. Stoppage in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:3509–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Wood CL, Warnell F, Johnson M, Hames A, Pearce MS, McConachie H, Parr JR. Evidence for ASD recurrence rates and reproductive stoppage from large UK ASD research family databases. Autism Res. 2015;8:73–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Rutter M, Macdonald H, Couteur A, Harrington R, Bolton P, Bailey A. Genetic factors in child psychiatric disorders—II. Empirical findings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;31:39–83.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Bolton P, Macdonald H, Pickles A, Rios PA, Goode S, Crowson M, Bailey A, Rutter M. A case-control family history study of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35:877–900.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B. Genetics of austim: complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nat Rev Genet. 2001;2:943–55.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Newschaffer CJ, Croen LA, Daniels J, Giarelli E, Grether JK, Levy SE, Mandell DS, Miller LA, Pinto-Martin J, Reaven J, et al. The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annual Review of Public Health. 2007;28:235–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Ludvigsson JF, Almqvist C, Bonamy AK, Ljung R, Michaelsson K, Neovius M, Stephansson O, Ye W. Registers of the Swedish total population and their use in medical research. Eur J Epidemiol. 2016;31:125–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ekbom A. The Swedish Multi-generation Register. Methods Mol Biol. 2011;675:215–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Ludvigsson JF, Andersson E, Ekbom A, Feychting M, Kim JL, Reuterwall C, Heurgren M, Olausson PO. External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Arlington: DSM-5; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  14. Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, Hultman C, Suren P, Susser E, Gronborg T, Gissler M, Gunnes N, Gross R, et al. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21:693–700.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Ludvigsson JF, Svedberg P, Olen O, Bruze G, Neovius M. The longitudinal integrated database for health insurance and labour market studies (LISA) and its use in medical research. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34(4):423–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Therneau TM, Grambsch PM. Modeling survival data: extending the Cox model. New York: Springer; 2000.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  18. Zeileis A. Object-oriented computation of sandwich estimators. J Stat Softw. 2006;16.

  19. Grambsch PM, Therneau TM. Proportional hazards tests and diagnostics based on weighted residuals. Biometrika. 1994;81:515–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Autismspektrumtillstånd. Autismspektrumtillstånd och ADHD bland barn och unga i Stockholms län: Förekomst i befolkningen samt vårdsökande under åren 2011 till 2016. Stockholms läns landsting: Centrum för epidemiologi och samhällsmedicin; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Idring S, Lundberg M, Sturm H, Dalman C, Gumpert C, Rai D, Lee BK, Magnusson C. Changes in prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in 2001-2011: findings from the Stockholm youth cohort. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:1766–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by The Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare; The Swedish Research Council; and The Beatrice and Samuel A. Seaver Foundation. Open access funding provided by Karolinska Institute.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

RKH, PL, and EF designed the study. HL and PL acquired the data. RKH and AC analyzed the data. All authors interpreted the analytic results. RKH and EF drafted the first manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ralf Kuja-Halkola.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was conducted on anonymized data, and the study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm, Sweden. As the study was register based, informed consent was waived by the Ethics Board.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

HL has served as a speaker for Evolan and Shire and has received a research grant from Shire, all outside the submitted work. SB has in the last 5 years acted as an author, consultant, or lecturer for Shire, Medice, Roche, Eli Lilly, Prima Psychiatry, GLGroup, System Analytic, Kompetento, Expo Medica, and Prophase. He receives royalties for text books and diagnostic tools from Huber/Hogrefe, Kohlhammer and UTB, all outside the submitted work. PL has served as a speaker for Medicine, all outside of submitted work.

No other disclosures are reported.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1: Figures S1–S3

and Table S1. Supplementary figures and tables.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kuja-Halkola, R., Larsson, H., Lundström, S. et al. Reproductive stoppage in autism spectrum disorder in a population of 2.5 million individuals. Molecular Autism 10, 45 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13229-019-0300-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13229-019-0300-6

Keywords