Skip to main content

Balanced bifrontal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances working memory in adults with high-functioning autism: a sham-controlled crossover study

Abstract

Background

Working memory (WM) often is impaired in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Such impairment may underlie core deficits in cognition and social functioning. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been shown to enhance WM in both healthy adults and clinical populations, but its efficacy in ASD is unknown. We predicted that bifrontal tDCS would improve WM performances of adults with high-functioning autism during active stimulation compared to sham stimulation and that such enhancement would generalize to an untrained task.

Methods

Twelve adults with high-functioning ASD engaged in a battery of WM tasks that included backward spatial span, backward digit span, spatial n-back and letter n-back. While engaged, 40 min of 1.5 mA bifrontal stimulation was applied over the left and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (DLPFC). Using a single-blind crossover design, each participant received left anodal/right cathodal stimulation, right anodal/left cathodal stimulation, or sham stimulation, in randomized counterbalanced order on three separate days. Following tDCS, participants again engaged in letter and spatial n-back tasks before taking the Brief Test of Attention (BTA). We used repeated-measures ANOVA to compare overall performance on the WM battery as measured by a composite of z-scores for all five measures. Post hoc ANOVAs, t tests, Friedman’s tests, and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were used to measure the online and offline effects of tDCS and to assess performances on individual measures.

Results

Compared to sham stimulation, both left DLPFC anodal stimulation (t 11 = 5.4, p = 0.0002) and right DLPFC anodal stimulation (t 11 = 3.57, p = 0.004) improved overall WM performance. Left anodal stimulation (t 11 = 3.9, p = 0.003) and right anodal stimulation (t 11 = 2.7, p = 0.019) enhanced performances during stimulation. Enhancement transferred to an untrained task 50 min after right anodal stimulation (z 11 = 2.263, p = 0.024). The tasks that showed the largest effects of active stimulation were spatial span backward (z 11 = 2.39, p = 0.017) and BTA (z 11 = 2.263, p = 0.024).

Conclusions

In adults with high-functioning ASD, active bifrontal tDCS given during WM tasks appears to improve performance. TDCS benefits also transferred to an untrained task completed shortly after stimulation. These results suggest that tDCS can improve WM task performance and could reduce some core deficits of autism.

Trial registration

NCT01602263

Background

Many individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show impairments in working memory (WM) [1,2,3]. Working memory refers to the capacity to maintain, update, and manipulate information held in temporary storage [4]. Poor WM performance has been shown in children and adolescents with ASD [5,6,7,8,9,10] and in adults with ASD [11,12,13]. Although other executive function deficits can resolve somewhat with age, WM deficits tend to persist [3, 14].

Working memory is critical for many complex cognitive functions including language [15, 16], general intelligence, and reasoning [17]; therefore, WM deficits likely produce profound effects in individuals with autism. Poor WM likely contributes to social problems in people with ASD [18] because maintaining continually changing social information in temporary storage (WM) is necessary for social flexibility [19]. Working memory also drives the ability to encode emotions observed on faces [20], regulate emotional responses [21], and break from restrictive or repetitive behaviors [22]. Therefore, remediating WM deficits could improve some of the core cognitive and behavioral deficits that characterize ASD.

Unfortunately, behavioral approaches to WM remediation have had limited effects [23], and attempted behavioral interventions have suffered from high attrition rates [24]. It also remains unclear whether improvements that may occur from behavioral remediation would generalize to other tasks or abilities [25]. Therefore, a simpler and faster approach to improving WM deficits might be more useful.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a non-invasive form of brain stimulation accomplished by the passage of a weak electrical current, typically in the range of 0.5–2.0 mA, through the scalp and skull via surface electrodes. The bases for tDCS effects have not been conclusively determined, but tDCS may modulate neural activity and behavior via several mechanisms, including alteration of membrane potentials [26], direct action at synaptic receptors [27], and downstream effects on network plasticity [28].

When applied over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), tDCS has enhanced verbal WM in healthy adults [29,30,31,32,33,34] and in patients with schizophrenia [35, 36], Parkinson’s disease [37], stroke [38], aphasia [39], and depression [40,41,42]. Anodal tDCS applied to the left prefrontal cortex in healthy adults has been shown to increase prefrontal cortex functional connectivity and to strengthen bilateral fronto-parietal networks [28]. Anodal tDCS to the right prefrontal cortex also has been shown to strengthen ipsilateral fronto-parietal connectivity and disrupt default mode network integrity [43], which potentially could be among the mechanisms for tDCS effects on WM.

Adults with ASD show prefrontal hypoactivation during working memory tasks [13, 44]. They also show reduced anterior-posterior connectivity [44,45,46]. Because WM depends on prefrontal activity [47] and communication between the DLPFC and posterior parietal resources [48], such deficits may provide at least a partial explanation for poor WM performance in individuals with ASD [49].

Therefore, there are both empirical and theoretical reasons for our primary aim, which is to determine whether tDCS applied over the prefrontal cortices improves WM in individuals with autism. The choice to use balanced bifrontal stimulation (F3-F4), rather than the more typical F3-right supraorbital montage or a unilateral montage, was motivated by several considerations. First, imaging and lesion studies suggest that WM is domain general [17, 50] and bilateral prefrontal resources are recruited for many WM tasks [51, 52]. Second, cathodal stimulation’s inhibitory effects on cognition are often weak or nonexistent [53, 54]; and in tasks that depend on bilateral resources, such effects may be countered by the effects of contralateral disinhibition. Third, balanced bilateral stimulation seems to distribute current more deeply [55] and broadly [56] and enhance functional connectivity more than what would be expected from the summed effects of the two electrodes due to interhemispheric interactions [57], which might contribute to a greater effect on widely distributed WM networks. Finally, when directly compared to unilateral montages or unbalanced bilateral montages, balanced bilateral stimulation (dual-hemisphere stimulation of homologous structures) has been shown to more effectively enhance motor learning [58, 59] and improve tactile discrimination [60]. Although no published studies or meta-analyses have directly compared the effectiveness of balanced bifrontal montages with unilateral or unbalanced bifrontal montages in the enhancement of WM, Richmond and colleagues [33] referred to pilot studies that showed that an F3-F4 montage was more effective than a F3-right supraorbital area montage. Balanced bifrontal montages have been shown to enhance WM accuracy [33, 35, 39,40,41,42, 56] with small to medium effect sizes in healthy adults [33, 56] and medium to large effect sizes in clinical populations [35, 39, 40, 42]. Data reported in meta-analyses suggest that unbalanced frontal stimulation (usually DLPFC-contralateral supraorbital area) tends to have a small effect on WM accuracy in both healthy populations [61] and clinical populations [62].

We explored two different active stimulation conditions because it is unknown whether the left or right DLPFC is a more effective target site for working memory enhancement in individuals with ASD. WM is not a monolithic mental function, but many of its sub-processes are mediated by activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex [63, 64]. Therefore, we used a short battery of WM tasks that require varying degrees of information maintenance and manipulation, and we used stimuli from different modalities. We explored an intervention in adults with ASD because WM deficits persist in ASD [3, 14] and because adults typically have aged out of behavioral modification programs and are developmentally stable.

We predicted that (1) applying active tDCS during WM training would enhance WM performance compared to sham stimulation, (2) material specificity of effects would not depend on the hemisphere stimulated by the anode, and (3) after stimulation, enhancement would transfer to an untrained task with a working memory component. We used composite z-scores as our primary outcome to capture overall WM enhancement across stimulus modalities.

Methods

Participants

The Institutional Review Board of The Johns Hopkins School of Medicine approved the study, and all participants gave written informed consent to participate. Twelve adults who were previously diagnosed with ASD were recruited from a Baltimore area residential and day center for adults with ASD. Their ages ranged from 20 to 66 years (M = 32.1; SD 12.4). They included 10 men and 2 women of whom 10 were Caucasian, 1 was African-American, and 1 was Asian. All participants had acquired language during childhood and spoke English as their first language. Potential subjects were excluded if their records or subsequent interviews suggested a history of neurological disease, psychiatric disorder, or active use of antipsychotic medications. The participants had IQ scores that ranged from 71 to 144 (M = 100.1; SD 23.1) on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test [65] and thus were classified as high functioning [1]. They had completed 12–21 years of schooling (M = 14.3; SD 3.1). Each study participant underwent a baseline assessment with the Autism Diagnosis Observation Schedule (ADOS) Module 4 [66], and the group was found to have a mean communication + social interaction score of 12.2 (SD 3.0), with scores of 4.2 (SD 1.4) on communication and 8 (SD 2.1) on social interaction. All participants met the ADOS cutoff of 7 for a diagnosis of ASD. All behavioral data were collected in a quiet room at The Johns Hopkins Hospital.

Test materials and tasks

Letter and spatial n-back

The n-back stimuli were displayed using E-Prime E Studio (v2.0 Psychology Software Tools, Inc.) on a Dell Inspiron N5110 with a 15.6-cm screen (diagonal length). Letter n-back stimuli were pseudo-randomly chosen from among the 20 consonants of the English alphabet. Letters were presented in the middle of the screen (10% screen height) in black bold Helvetica on a white background. During spatial n-back, a constant 1-cm thick blue fixation cross was extended to the full width and height of the screen. Blue rectangles appeared pseudo-randomly in one of the four quadrants, covering 75% of the available space in the quadrant. For both letter and spatial n-back tasks, stimuli were displayed for 500 ms with a 2000-ms inter-stimulus interval. Participants indicated whether the stimuli were the same or different than the previous stimuli by pressing buttons on the keyboard. If participants did not respond within 2500 ms after stimulus onset, a fixation cross appeared, followed by the next stimulus. The load for each block increased from 1-back to 2-back to 3-back. At the top of the screen, a reminder message identified the current task load, e.g., 2-back. Brief visual instructions were presented prior to every change in load to show what was meant by 1-back, 2-back, or 3-back. All participants completed 3-minute practice blocks that included 12 trials at each load at the beginning of the stimulation period. On the practice trials, response accuracy feedback was provided: The screen flashed green for 50 ms for a correct response or red for an error or failure to respond within 2500 ms. Participants were told that failure to respond would be counted as incorrect. During stimulation, each participant completed one 12-min block of 198 spatial n-back trials and one 12-min block of 198 letter n-back trials (each block consisting of 66 trials at loads of 1-, 2-, and 3-back) to assess the online effects of tDCS. Following the cessation of stimulation, each participant also completed two 6-min blocks of 102 trials (each with 34 trials at loads of 1-, 2-, and 3-back) to assess the offline effects of tDCS on WM. N-back accuracy was calculated as (hits + correct negatives)/(total items). The n-back tasks are depicted in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Letter and spatial n-back task procedures

Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition (WMS-III)

The WMS-III digit span backward and spatial span backward tasks were administered as per the test manual [67]. Briefly, participants repeated strings of digits in reverse order for digit span backward and tapped a sequence of blocks on a board in reverse order for spatial span backward. Forward digit span and forward spatial span tasks were omitted.

The Brief Test of Attention (BTA)

The BTA was given as per the instruction manual [68] except that each participant received only one form (i.e., either numbers or letters) as determined by computer-generated random number assignment. Participants listened to 10 strings of letters and numbers (e.g., F-3-7-R-4-2-Q) and were required to ignore the numbers (or letters) and to continually rely on working memory to update a tally of how many letters (or numbers) were read aloud in each string. The raw score is the total number of correctly tallied strings, ranging from 0 to 10. All items in the subtest were administered regardless of performance.

Stimulation side-effect questionnaire

On a 16-item side effects questionnaire that we developed based on references to side effects in the literature [69,70,71], participants rated their experiences of headache, difficulty with concentration, discomfort, fatigue, pain, tingling, nausea, anxiety, anger, confusion, happiness, sadness, tension, fear, alertness, and vision changes. Participants circled a number on a Likert scale ranging from 0 to 10 (0 = absent; 10 = most extreme). Subjects were given this questionnaire before and after each tDCS session. At the end of the questionnaire given after each session, the participants were asked to indicate (yes/no) whether they thought that they had just received real stimulation [72].

tDCS stimulation parameters

Transcranial direct current stimulation was delivered by a neuroConn DC-stimulator PLUS (http://www.neurocaregroup.com/dc_stimulator_plus.html). In all conditions, two flat carbon rubber electrodes (surface area 25 cm2) encased in saline-soaked sponges were placed over F3 and F4 using the 10–20 international electrode positioning system. The electrodes were secured evenly over the scalp with 1-in. wide rubber straps: One strap was parallel to the horizontal plane and the second strap was anchored to the first strap and arched over F3 and F4. Electrode sponges were aligned so that the bottom sides were parallel with the strap wrapped around the horizontal plane. The active stimulation conditions were designated according to the location of the anodal electrode for the sake of brevity, not to imply that the anode was more important than the cathode or that their effects would be distinguished from one another. There were three conditions:

  1. 1)

    Left anodal: The anode was over F3 and the cathode over F4. Stimulation was ramped up from 0 to 1.5 mA on a sinus curve for 15 s, held constant at 1.5 mA (current density = 0.06 mA/cm2) for 39.5 min, and ramped back down to 0 mA on a sinus curve for 15 s.

  2. 2)

    Right anodal: The anode was over F4 and the cathode over F3. Stimulation was ramped up from 0 to 1.5 mA on a sinus curve for 15 s, held constant at 1.5 mA (current density = 0.06 mA/cm2) for 39.5 min, and ramped back down to 0 mA on a sinus curve for 15 s.

  3. 3)

    Sham: The anode was over F3 and the cathode over F4. Stimulation was ramped up from 0 to 1.5 mA on a sinus curve for 15 s, held constant at 1.5 mA for 30 s, and ramped back down to 0 mA on a sinus curve for 15 s.

Procedures

The sequence of procedures is indicated in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Experimental protocol. All participants completed the same protocol on three separate days. They received either left anodal stimulation, right anodal stimulation, or sham stimulation in counterbalanced order

In a 3-session crossover design, participants were randomly assigned via computerized random number generator to one of six groups with order of condition counterbalanced (left anodal, right anodal, sham). Group sizes were evenly balanced. Participants were given the KBIT-2 [65] prior to session 1. Before each tDCS session, they were scanned in a Siemens 3 T MRI for 13 min (results not reported here, but see [73]). Thereafter, the tDCS device was affixed and participants completed the side-effects questionnaire prior to stimulation. During stimulation, participants engaged in 8 min of test explanations and practice, followed by digit span backward and spatial span backward tasks, 12 min of letter n-back problems, and 12 min of spatial n-back problems. After the 40-min period of active or sham stimulation ended, participants again completed the side-effects questionnaire and the tDCS apparatus was removed. Then, following an approximately 40-min interval during which resting-state fMRI data again were collected, participants completed the offline n-back tasks, as described above, followed by the BTA. Other than stimulation parameters, the protocol was identical in all three sessions. Sessions were separated by a mean of 6.8 (5.1) days, with a minimum washout of 24 h between sessions. Testing was single blind; the participants were not aware of the test conditions, although the technicians were.

Analyses

With a one-way repeated measures design, we tested our hypothesis that tDCS would enhance overall WM performance during and after active stimulation compared to sham stimulation by examining performance on a cognitive test battery. As our primary outcome measures, we computed composite z-scores for each participant. One reason that composite z-scores were used in the primary analysis was because the number of participants was suboptimal with regard to power, and the participants were anticipated to vary widely in IQ and education. Composite z-scores are an effective tool to evaluate performance on a battery of tests that recruit resources from a common cognitive domain [74,75,76] because they increase statistical power by attenuating floor and ceiling effects, they reduce random variability, and they lower the number of correlated data in analyses [77]. Participants’ raw scores were transformed to z-scores using all 36 data points (12 participants × 3 sessions) obtained for each test. The full-battery composite z-score was the mean of the participant’s z-scores for all five working memory tests: WMS-III spatial span backward (longest length), WMS-III digit span backward (longest length), verbal n-back accuracy (mean of online and offline scores), spatial n-back accuracy (mean of online and offline scores), and BTA (raw score). We also computed an online composite z-score for the four online measures of working memory and an offline composite z-score for the three offline measures (see Figs. 2 and 3e), based on the fact that BTA performance requires a significant working memory component (updating). Measures of working memory capacity and accuracy were chosen, rather than reaction time, because those measures have been correlated with behaviors related to core deficits in autism [20,21,22] and because tDCS tends to enhance accuracy more than reaction time in clinical populations [78]. The main effect of stimulation on overall WM performance (full-battery composite z-score) was analyzed with repeated-measures ANOVA, with stimulation condition as the independent factor, followed by post hoc, paired-sample t tests. We compared the online composite z-scores across conditions with repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc, paired-sample t tests. We compared the offline composite z-scores across conditions with Friedman’s and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests because the data were not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk, p = 0.045). For secondary analyses, we examined the effects of active vs. sham stimulation on individual WM measures in native space using repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc t tests or with Friedman’s and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. We also tested the effectiveness of blinding study participants to the stimulation conditions as measured by self-report. All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) for Windows, Version 22.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Effect sizes for paired-sample t tests were Cohen’s d s  = mean difference/pooled SD. Effect sizes for Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were r = z/√(2*N). For the purpose of comparing the effects reported here to those in other studies, we provided Cohen’s d s effect sizes for all significant analyses.

Fig. 3
figure 3

a z-scores for individual tests and composite z-scores. Composite z-score (online) is the mean of four z-scores: spatial span backward maximum length, digit span backward maximum length, online letter n-back accuracy, and online spatial n-back accuracy. Composite z-score (offline) is the mean of three z-scores: offline letter n-back accuracy, offline spatial n-back accuracy, and BTA raw score. Composite z-score (full battery) is the mean of five z-scores: spatial span backward maximum length, digit span backward maximum length, letter n-back accuracy (mean of online and offline percentages), spatial n-back accuracy (mean of online and offline percentages), and BTA raw score (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001). b Backward spatial span, backward digit span, and Brief Test of Attention task results. Error bars indicate standard deviations. (*p < .05). c N-back accuracy. Percentage correct for online spatial n-back, online letter n-back, offline spatial n-back, and offline letter n-back. Error bars indicate standard deviations. d Individual differences in full-battery composite z-scores (vs. sham) for left anodal and right anodal stimulation. Mean composite z-scores (full battery) were −0.25 (SD 0.71) for sham stimulation, 0.13 (SD 0.82) for left anodal stimulation, and 0.11 (SD 0.81) for right anodal stimulation

Results

Primary outcome: overall WM performance

As shown in the final row of Table 1, the composite z-scores (full battery) were −0.25 (SD 0.71) for sham stimulation, 0.13 (SD 0.82) for left anodal stimulation, and 0.11 (SD 0.81) for right anodal stimulation, which repeated measures ANOVA showed to be significantly different (F 2,22 = 12.85, p = 0.0002). Post hoc, paired-sample t tests revealed that performances associated with both left anodal active stimulation (t 11 = 5.4, p = 0.0002) and right anodal active stimulation (t 11 = 3.57, p = 0.004) were better than performances associated with sham tDCS. For composite z-scores (full battery), Cohen’s d s effect sizes were 0.50 for left anodal stimulation and 0.47 for right anodal stimulation. There was no difference in overall WM performance between the two active stimulation conditions (t 11 = 0.26, p = 0.796). To assess the role of session order on full-battery composite z-scores, we compared them across sessions. We found composite z-scores (full battery) of −0.03 (SD 0.81) for session 1, −0.06 (SD 0.75) for session 2, and 0.09 (SD 0.83) for session 3. Repeated measures ANOVA showed no significant practice effects across sessions (F2,22 = 0.88, p = 0.428).

Table 1 Behavioral data and statistics: Composite z-score (online) is the mean of four z-scores: spatial span backward maximum length, digit span backward maximum length, online letter n-back accuracy, and online spatial n-back accuracy. Composite z-score (offline) is the mean of three z-scores: offline letter n-back accuracy, offline spatial n-back accuracy, and BTA raw score. Composite z-score (full battery) is the mean of five z-scores: spatial span backward maximum length, digit span backward maximum length, letter n-back accuracy (mean of online and offline percentages), spatial n-back accuracy (mean of online and offline percentages), and BTA raw score. Also included are means and standard deviations for 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back for letter and spatial n-back, both online and offline

The composite z-scores (online) were −0.25 (SD 0.74) for sham stimulation, 0.15 (SD 0.87) for left anodal stimulation, and 0.09 (SD 0.81) for right anodal stimulation, which repeated measures ANOVA showed to be significantly different (F 2,22 = 7.68, p = 0.003). Post hoc, paired-sample t tests revealed that performances as measured by composite z-scores of the four working memory tests given during both left anodal active stimulation (t 11 = 3.9, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d s  = 0.52) and right anodal active stimulation (t 11 = 2.7, p = 0.019, Cohen’s d s  = 0.46) were better than performances during sham stimulation. Performances during the two active stimulation conditions (t 11 = 0.60, p = 0.563) were not significantly different.

The composite z-scores (offline) were −0.18 (SD 0.92) for sham stimulation, 0.09 (SD 0.77) for left anodal stimulation, and 0.09 (SD 0.93) for right anodal stimulation, which Friedman’s test showed to be significantly different (χ2 = 7.17, p = 0.028). Follow-up Wilcoxon signed-rank tests showed that after left anodal stimulation performances were not significantly better compared to after sham stimulation (z = 1.88, p = 0.06, r = 0.27, Cohen’s d s  = 0.33), but performances after right anodal stimulation were significantly better than after sham stimulation (z = 2.35, p = 0.019, r = 0.34, Cohen’s d s  = 0.30). Although performances after right anodal stimulation were marginally better than after left anodal stimulation, the difference was not significant (z = 1.49, p = 0.136).

Secondary outcomes: individual WM measures

Mean (SD) scores for each of the five measures that made up our overall WM composite are presented in Table 1. As shown in the first row, the participants’ longest backward spatial span was 4.8 (SD 0.9) blocks during sham stimulation compared to 6.2 (SD 1.8) blocks during left anodal stimulation and 6.0 (SD 0.9) blocks during right anodal stimulation. Friedman’s test showed that these differed significantly (χ2 = 9.24, p = 0.010). Post hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank tests indicate that left anodal stimulation (z 11 = 2.21, p = 0.027, r = 0.32) and right anodal stimulation (z 11 = 2.39, p = 0.017, r = 0.34) were both associated with longer maximum spans than sham stimulation. Cohen’s d s effect sizes were 1.03 for left anodal stimulation and 1.39 for right anodal stimulation. Performances during the two active stimulation conditions were not different (z 11 = 0.30, p = 0.762). We examined the role of practice effects on all tests for which significant differences were found between stimulation conditions. Participants’ longest spatial span was 5.6 blocks (SD 1.2) during session 1, 5.5 blocks (SD 1.1) during session 2, and 5.9 blocks (SD 1.8) during session 3. Friedman tests showed no significant practice effects across sessions (χ2 = 0.05, p = 0.973).

Also as shown in Table 1, participants’ longest backward digit span averaged 5.3 (SD 1.5) digits during sham compared to 5.5 (SD 1.6) digits during left anodal and 5.4 (SD 2.0) during right anodal stimulation. Friedman’s test showed no significant differences in longest digit span between conditions (χ2 = 0.41, p = 0.814).

Spatial n-back accuracy was calculated as (hits + correct negatives)/(total items). Online n-back accuracy was 62.4% (SD 25.0%) during sham stimulation, 68.6% (SD 22.1%) during left anodal stimulation, and 66.7% (SD 23.2%) during right anodal stimulation. Friedman’s test showed no significant differences between conditions (χ2 = 3.17, p = 0.205). Offline spatial n-back accuracy was 63.9% (SD 24.9%) after sham stimulation, 70.6% (SD 20.9%) after left anodal stimulation, and 67.7% (SD 24.2%) after right anodal stimulation, which were not significantly different (χ2 = 0.55, p = 0.758).

Letter n-back accuracy was calculated as (hits + correct negatives)/(total items). Online n-back accuracy was 69.6% (SD 30.2%) during sham stimulation, 76.2% (SD 22.5%) during left anodal stimulation, and 76.3% (SD 26.8%) during right anodal stimulation. These were not significantly different (χ2 = 0.5, p = 0.779). Offline letter n-back accuracy was 72.3% (SD 29.8%) after sham stimulation, 78.5% (SD 22.5%) after left anodal stimulation, and 76.5% (SD 25.9%) after right anodal stimulation. These also were not significantly different (χ2 = 0.17, p = 0.920).

Brief Test of Attention (BTA) raw scores averaged 6.8 (SD 2.5) after sham stimulation compared to 7.4 (SD 2.2) after left anodal and 7.9 (SD 2.5) after right anodal stimulation. We found the predicted main effect of condition on BTA performance with Freidman’s test (χ2 = 7.09, p = 0.029). Follow-up Wilcoxon signed-rank tests revealed that BTA performance after left anodal stimulation was not significantly different from BTA performance after sham stimulation (z 11 = 1.63, p = 0.102, r = 0.24, Cohen’s d s  = 0.27), but BTA performance after right anodal stimulation was better than after sham (z 11 = 2.26, p = 0.024, r = 0.33, Cohen’s d s  = 0.46). Additionally, BTA performance was better after right than left anodal stimulation (z 11 = 2.45, p = 0.014, r = 0.35, Cohen’s d s  = 0.22). There was no effect of practice on BTA performance from session 1 to session 3 (z 11 = −1.12, p = 0.265). The results of our secondary analyses are depicted in Fig. 3a–c.

Blinding

We hypothesized that participants would tolerate tDCS and remain blind to condition. All participants completed the three sessions with no adverse events. After each session, all 12 participants were asked to indicate (yes/no) whether they thought they had received real stimulation. Eleven participants endorsed left anodal stimulation as real, nine endorsed right anodal stimulation as real, and 10 endorsed sham as real. McNemar’s test of related samples showed no differences in endorsement rates between sham and both left (p > 0.99) and right (p > 0.99) active anodal stimulation.

Discussion

In this study, the effects of tDCS on some WM tasks are among the largest in the literature. Meta-analyses of attempts to enhance working memory have typically shown smaller enhancing effects of anodal stimulation at F3 in both healthy adults and clinical populations [61, 62]. We observed a Cohen’s d s of 0.50 for full-battery WM performance (as measured by composite z-scores) associated with left anodal stimulation compared to sham stimulation and 0.47 for right anodal stimulation compared to sham stimulation. The measures on which we observed the largest significant effects of tDCS were backward spatial span (d s  = 1.33) and the Brief Test of Attention (d s  = 0.46). These effect sizes are similar in magnitude to those reported in other investigations of balanced bifrontal stimulation to enhance WM [33, 39,40,41,42]. Although enhanced performance was not significant on some individual WM tasks in the battery, group performance associated with either type of active stimulation was at least equal to and in most cases, better than sham on all tasks (see Fig. 3a) and most of the participants showed improved composite z-scores in both active conditions (see Fig. 3d).

The larger than typical effect sizes reported here could be attributed to several factors. WM deficits in individuals with ASD might provide more room for improvement than in healthy adults. Also, we used a balanced bifrontal montage, which not only capitalizes on the direct effects of anodal stimulation but also likely combines them with the effects of contralateral disinhibition, as well as a tendency to recruit additional aspects of working memory networks [57]. Additionally, stimulation was delivered during WM performance, which seems to produce the most consistent effects [61]. Finally, our participants received active tDCS at a current density of 0.06 mA/cm2 for 40 min. This is twice as long as it has been typical of behavioral studies, although periods of 30 min and longer [32, 38, 40, 41, 79,80,81,82,83] are becoming common in recent years, especially in studies of cognition. Longer stimulation periods seem to more effectively modulate WM [62], current density greater than 0.029 mA/cm2 is positively correlated with greater effects on WM [62], and total charge delivered is positively correlated with effect size in studies of cognition [78].

Two characteristics of tDCS are particularly intriguing when considering its potential for cognitive enhancement: (1) its functional resolution and (2) its broad distribution of current. Functional resolution is the tendency of tDCS to alter synaptic plasticity preferentially in networks that are undergoing task-related activity during stimulation [84,85,86]. Broad distribution of current has been demonstrated via electroencephalography [87, 88], fMRI [28, 89,90,91,92], and positron emission tomography [92], all of which have been used to show that tDCS alters cortical activity both beneath the electrodes and in more remote regions. Our two current flow simulations suggested that with the balanced bifrontal montage, current flow is not limited to the DLPFC, but is broadly distributed to prefrontal cortical regions as well as to subcortical regions via white matter tracts (see Fig. 4). Wide current distribution and functional resolution could be particularly advantageous for WM enhancement given the broad topology of WM networks and heterogeneous etiologies of WM dysfunction.

Fig. 4
figure 4

a Simulation of 1.0 mA current flow with an F3 anodal-F4 cathodal montage from Soterix TDCS Explore. Maximum field intensity is 0.25 V/m. Current is disbursed throughout the frontal cortex. b Simulation of 1.0 mA current flow with an F3 anodal-F4 cathodal montage from http://neuralengr.com/bonsai/. Maximum field intensity is 0.306 V/m. Current flows throughout frontal cortex and penetrates to subcortical areas

The broadly distributed current and functional resolution of tDCS prompt us to consider several possible mechanisms of action rather than restricting ourselves to the oversimplified view that the enhancement of WM found in this study is due solely to improved maintenance and manipulation of information driven by neuromodulation of the DLPFC.

TDCS also may have increased attentional control of WM by improving top-down signaling from the prefrontal cortex. Functional MRI studies of healthy adults show a diffuse WM network characterized by core attentional control nodes in bilateral DLPFC [93]. When WM demand is low, individuals with ASD and typically developing controls both rely on bottom-up parietal processes to direct external attention. As loads increase, prefrontal activity increases in typically developing controls but not persons with ASD, suggesting a failure to exert sufficient top-down control of attentional resources [49]. In ASD, reduced anterior-posterior connectivity [44, 45] likely contributes to the PFC’s poor integration with the rest of the WM network. Anodal tDCS over DLPFC can strengthen anterior-posterior connectivity in dorsolateral-parietal attentional networks [28, 43], which may have temporarily improved attentional control mediated by the PFC. Also, Park and colleagues [94] showed that following a stroke, 30 min of anodal stimulation over left DLPFC enhanced subsequent performance on the Auditory Continuous Performance Test, a test of attention and response control on which individuals with ASD do poorly [95]. Our finding of enhancement on a task with both working memory and attentional components (BTA) further supports improved attentional control as a potential mechanism for performance enhancement.

Another possible benefit of tDCS could be improved emotional regulation mediated by the right DLPFC [96], which is hypoactive in ASD [46]. Better emotional control could help participants combat frustration as WM loads increase [97] and resist the effects of stress [98]. Enhanced PFC integration with the rest of the WM network may also improve the ability to suppress irrelevant information [99] and ignore interference [100].

Yet another possibility is that tDCS improves vigilance. Using the same balanced bifrontal montage, Nelson and colleagues [101] showed enhanced vigilance on an air traffic control simulator as measured by better discrimination (A’) following left anodal stimulation compared to sham. Executive control of vigilance has been linked to the right PFC for easier tasks and to bilateral PFC for more difficult tasks. Here, we found that both left and right anodal stimulation improved overall WM and that BTA performance improved after right compared to left anodal stimulation. The errorless BTA performance of a few participants, even after sham stimulation, suggests that this task was less demanding than the others, which could explain the relatively greater benefit of right vs. left anodal stimulation.

Current flow simulations and studies using fMRI [102] indicate that during balanced bifrontal stimulation, current passes through the anterior cingulate. The anterior cingulate cortex is activated for many types of executive function tasks, including vigilance, performance monitoring, and error avoidance. Individual variability in WM dysfunction and the theorized functional resolution of tDCS suggest that the same tDCS montage could strengthen response inhibition in one person, improve response monitoring in another, and improve vigilance in a third person.

Given traditional assumptions of an excitatory role for anodal stimulation and an inhibitory role for cathodal stimulation, as well as previous research suggesting right anodal stimulation of the DLPFC is less effective [61], the finding that both active bifrontal montages were similarly effective in enhancing performance may seem counter-intuitive. There is growing evidence that the canonical assumptions of anodal excitation and cathodal inhibition beneath the electrodes are often violated [53, 54]. Also, direction of current flow may begin to matter less as amperage increases from 1 to 2 mA [103], which would suggest that the montages are more equivalent in their effects at areas more remote from the electrodes, such as the anterior cingulate. Several of the potential mechanisms for enhancement described in the preceding paragraphs could be triggered by either left anodal stimulation or right anodal stimulation. For example, both bifrontal montages could have increased anterior-posterior connectivity that is typically reduced in working memory networks [44, 49, 104], and both montages would have passed current through the anterior cingulate, which is desynchronized from other aspects of working memory networks [105] and which shows abnormal activity related to attentional and executive control in individuals with ASD. Finally, right anodal stimulation also may have ameliorated the poor emotional regulation typically driven by right prefrontal hypoactivity in ASD.

The findings reported here are limited in several ways, which may inform improvements in future approaches. First, given that mean scores on the BTA approached the maximum for this test, ceiling effects might have obscured the benefit of active tDCS on BTA performance. A more difficult version of this task might yield a better estimate of effect size. Also, some higher-functioning participants showed ceiling effects on n-back accuracy rates, even at 2-back and 3-back, while lower-performing participants showed improvements with stimulation. Stimulation may enhance performance only when recipients are working near capacity. Thus, future investigations could titrate participants’ n-back load until previously specified performance criteria are met [33, 106]. It is also likely that the 50-min delay between the end of stimulation and the start of offline performances allowed for the effects of stimulation to diminish, so immediate offline effects of stimulation may be larger than those reported here. Although the 8-min practice time ensured that all outcomes were measured after sufficient stimulation had been delivered to alter synaptic plasticity [26, 107], the additive impact of tDCS duration on cognitive performance is unknown. Some studies show increasing effects with longer periods of stimulation [62, 108], while others show that longer periods of stimulation can shrink or even reverse effects [109]. Overall, task order did not predict effect size; the largest effects of stimulation were on the second task (spatial span backward) and the last task (BTA). The mean intersession interval was 6.8 (5.1) days, but for two participants, the interval was only 24 h. Although the effects of tDCS (relative to sham) on acquired motor skills and other learning have been shown to persist for months or longer with repeated training [106, 110, 111], we are not aware of any studies that show a single session of tDCS continuing to alter behavior or secondary measures of neural activity, such as regional cerebral blood flow, blood oxygenation, or neurotransmitter release, more than 24 h after stimulation ends. However, findings in the TMS literature have demonstrated hyperplasticity in individuals with ASD, such that motor cortical plasticity endures 2–3 times longer than in healthy controls [112, 113]. Although the combination of a 40-min stimulation period and ASD hyperplasticity could increase the possibility of carryover effects of active stimulation, offline effects would still not be expected to persist to the next day and affect subsequent performance. Also, with multiple active conditions, intersession intervals should ideally be standardized to at least 1 week to avoid metaplasticity that has been shown to endure for at least 24 h in individuals with ASD who receive theta-burst stimulation [113]. Similar effects have also been observed in repeated tDCS to enhance working memory, with training gains smaller when stimulation sessions are separated by a 24-h interval compared to a 3-day interval [106]. The effects of both hyperplasticity and metaplasticity could have reduced power; hyperplasticity could have carried over to subsequent sham performance and metaplasticity could have attenuated the effects of next-day stimulation. Such effects would likely diminish differences between active stimulation and sham. Although washout period did not predict changes in composite z-scores between sessions with the same order of conditions, the possibility of an interaction between condition and washout period cannot be precluded because of insufficient power to detect even a large effect.

Our results are novel in several ways. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of tDCS as a tool to enhance WM in individuals with ASD, which is important because WM deficits likely contribute to impairments of more complex cognitive processes and social behavior in those individuals. This also is the first report of tDCS-mediated enhancement of spatial WM using a balanced bifrontal montage. In addition, our administration of tDCS for 40 min is the longest reported period of bifrontal tDCS. Recent reviews suggest that spatial WM is more impaired than verbal WM in individuals with ASD [2], which may explain why we found larger effects of prefrontal tDCS on spatial WM performance than have previously been reported in healthy adults [100, 114].

Finally, we found that the beneficial effects of tDCS on subsequent performance persisted for at least 50 min after stimulation, which is the longest delay between stimulation and an offline effect of stimulation on WM-driven task performance of which we are aware. Previously, offline effects on subsequent WM task performance have been found to persist for as long as 30–40 min [32, 36, 38, 115] after stimulation. Clearly, longer follow-up monitoring is needed to assess effect persistence after stimulation and to attempt more enduring facilitation of WM with repeated sessions of stimulation and training. It is also worth noting that persisting offline effects of tDCS transferred to another test with a working memory component on which participants did not train during stimulation. This is consistent with previous findings of near transfer [29, 33, 85, 116, 117]. Future experiments should explore transfer from laboratory-based tasks to applied cognitive and social skills that enhance quality of life.

Conclusions

We found that a balanced bifrontal tDCS montage can enhance both online and offline WM performance in adults with high-functioning autism. The largest effects were on spatial span and BTA performance. Left anodal stimulation and right anodal stimulation both enhanced WM, with effects ranging from small to large. Material-specific effects of anodal stimulation were not observed over either hemisphere. As hypothesized, online stimulation effects transferred to an untrained offline task with a working memory component, but enhancement (compared to sham) reached significance after right anodal stimulation only.

In summary, our pilot study suggests that tDCS shows promise as a method to enhance WM in adults with high-functioning ASD. Further studies are needed to replicate these effects in a larger sample and determine if repeated stimulation can produce lasting effects that transfer to real-world skills.

Abbreviations

ADOS:

Autism Diagnosis Observation Schedule

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

BTA:

Brief Test of Attention

DLPFC:

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

K-BIT2:

Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, 2nd Edition

PFC:

Prefrontal cortex

tDCS:

Transcranial direct current stimulation

WM:

Working memory

WMS-III:

Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition

References

  1. Barendse EM, Hendriks MP, Jansen JF, Backes WH, Hofman PA, Thoonen G, et al. Working memory deficits in high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: neuropsychological and neuroimaging correlates. J Neurodev Disord. 2013;5(1):14–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Kercood S, Grskovic JA, Banda D, Begeske J. Working memory and autism: a review of literature. Res Autism Spect Dis. 2014;8(10):1316–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Wang YZ, Zhang YB, Liu L-L, Cui J-F, Wang J, DHK S, van Amelsvoort T, RCK C. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev. 2017;27(1):46–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol. 2012;63:1–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Corbett BA, Constantine LJ, Hendren R, Rocke D, Ozonoff S. Examining executive functioning in children with autism spectrum disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and typical development. Psychiatry Res. 2009;166(2-3):210–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. WISC-IV and WIAT-II profiles in children with high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(3):428–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Verte S, Geurts HM, Roeyers H, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. The relationship of working memory, inhibition, and response variability in child psychopathology. J Neurosci Methods. 2006;151(1):5–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Jiang YV, Capistrano CG, Palm BE. Spatial working memory in children with high-functioning autism: intact configural processing but impaired capacity. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(1):248–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bennetto L, Pennington BF, Rogers SJ. Intact and impaired memory functions in autism. Child Dev. 1996;67(4):1816–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Steele SD, Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA. Spatial working memory deficits in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(4):605–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bodner KE, Beversdorf DQ, Saklayen SS, Christ SE. Noradrenergic moderation of working memory impairments in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(3):556–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Williams DL, Goldstein G, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ. Verbal and spatial working memory in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(6):747–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Luna B, Minshew NJ, Garver KE, Lazar NA, Thulborn KR, Eddy WF, et al. Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: an fMRI study of spatial working memory. Neurology. 2002;59(6):834–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Geurts HM, Vissers ME. Elderly with autism: executive functions and memory. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(5):665–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Joseph RM, McGrath LM, Tager-Flusberg H. Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in verbal school-age children with autism. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;27(3):361–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Baddeley A. Working memory and language: an overview. J Commun Disord. 2003;36(3):189–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The generality of working memory capacity: a latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004;133(2):189–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Gilotty L, Kenworthy L, Sirian L, Black DO, Wagner AE. Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8(4):241–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Meyer ML, Spunt RP, Berkman ET, Taylor SE, Lieberman MD. Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(6):1883–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Phillips LH, Channon S, Tunstall M, Hedenstrom A, Lyons K. The role of working memory in decoding emotions. Emotion. 2008;8(2):184–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Schmeichel BJ, Volokhov RN, Dernaree HA. Working memory capacity and the self-regulation of emotional expression and experience. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;95(6):1526–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Lopez BR, Lincoln AJ, Ozonoff S, Lai Z. Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(4):445–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Is working memory training effective? Psychol Bull. 2012;138(4):628–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. de Vries M, Prins PJ, Schmand BA, Geurts HM. Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:566–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Shipstead Z, Redick TS, Engle RW. Does working memory training generalize? Psychol Belg. 2010;50(3-4):245–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Nitsche MA, Cohen LG, Wassermann EM, Priori A, Lang N, Antal A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 2008;1(3):206–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Brunoni AR, Ferrucci R, Fregni F, Boggio PS, Priori A. Transcranial direct current stimulation for the treatment of major depressive disorder: a summary of preclinical, clinical and translational findings. Prog Neuro-Psychoph. 2012;39(1):9–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Keeser D, Meindl T, Bor J, Palm U, Pogarell O, Mulert C, et al. Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation changes connectivity of resting-state networks during fMRI. J Neurosci. 2011;31(43):15284–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Andrews SC, Hoy KE, Enticott PG, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzgerald PB. Improving working memory: the effect of combining cognitive activity and anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Stimul. 2011;4(2):84–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Fregni F, Boggio PS, Nitsche M, Bermpohl F, Antal A, Feredoes E, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp Brain Res. 2005;166(1):23–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hoy KE, Emonson MRL, Arnold SL, Thomson RH, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzgerald PB. Testing the limits: investigating the effect of tDCS dose on working memory enhancement in healthy controls. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(9):1777–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Ohn SH, Parkd C-I, Yooe W-K, Kof M-H, Choia KP, Kimb G-M, et al. Time-dependent effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the enhancement of working memory. Neuroreport. 2008;19(8):43–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Richmond LL, Wolk D, Chein J, Olson IR. Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances verbal working memory training performance over time and near transfer outcomes. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014;26(11):2443–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Zaehle T, Sandmann P, Thorne JD, Jancke L, Herrmann CS. Transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex modulates working memory performance: combined behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. BMC Neurosci. 2011;12:2–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Schretlen DJ, van Steenburgh JJ, Varvaris M, Vannorsdall TD, Andrejczuk MA, Gordon B. Can transcranial direct current stimulation improve cognitive functioning in adults with schizophrenia? Clin Schizophr Relat Psychoses. 2014:1–27. [Epub ahead of print].

  36. Hoy KE, Arnold SL, Emonson MRL, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzgerald PB. An investigation into the effects of tDCS dose on cognitive performance over time in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2014;155(1-3):96–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Boggio PS, Ferrucci R, Rigonatti SP, Covre P, Nitsche M, Pascual-Leone A, et al. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006;249(1):31–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Jo JM, Kim YH, Ko MH, Ohn SH, Joen B, Lee KH. Enhancing the working memory of stroke patients using tDCS. Am J Phys Med Rehab. 2009;88(5):404–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Saidmanesh MP, Hamid R, Amini A, Nillipour R, Ekhtiari H. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients with non-fluent aphasia disorder. Res J Biological Sci. 2012;7(7):290–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Oliveira JF, Zanão TA, Valiengo L, Lotufo PA, Benseñor IM, Fregni F, et al. Acute working memory improvement after tDCS in antidepressant-free patients with major depressive disorder. Neurosci Lett. 2013;537:60–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Moreno ML, Vanderhasselt MA, Carvalho AF, Moffa AH, Lotufo PA, Bensenor IM, et al. Effects of acute transcranial direct current stimulation in hot and cold working memory tasks in healthy and depressed subjects. Neurosci Lett. 2015;591:126–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Salehinejad MA, Rostami R, Ghanavati E. Transcranial direct current stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of major depression: improving visual working memory. Reducing Depressive Symptoms NeuroRegulation. 2015;2(1):37.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Pena-Gomez C, Sala-Lonch R, Junque C, Clemente IC, Vidal D, Bargallo N, et al. Modulation of large-scale brain networks by transcranial direct current stimulation evidenced by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Stimulation. 2012;5(3):252–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Koshino H, Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA. fMRI investigation of working memory for faces in autism: visual coding and underconnectivity with frontal areas. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(2):289–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cherkassky VL, Kana RK, Keller TA, Just MA. Functional connectivity in a baseline resting-state network in autism. Neuroreport. 2006;17(16):1687–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Kana RK, Keller TA, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(3):198–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Rypma B. Prefrontal cortical contributions to working memory: evidence from event-related fMRI studies. Exp Brain Res. 2000;133(1):3–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Cohen JD, Perlstein WM, Braver TS, Nystrom LE, Noll DC, Jonides J, et al. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature. 1997;386(6625):604–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Vogan VM, Morgan BR, Lee W, Powell TL, Smith ML, Taylor MJ. The neural correlates of visuo-spatial working memory in children with autism spectrum disorder: effects of cognitive load. J Neurodev Disord. 2014;6(1):19–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Barbey AK, Koenigs M, Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex. 2013;49(5):1195–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):46–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Nystrom LE, Braver TS, Sabb FW, Delgado MR, Noll DC, Cohen JD. Working memory for letters, shapes, and locations: fMRI evidence against stimulus-based regional organization in human prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2000;11(5):424–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Jacobson L, Koslowsky M, Lavidor M. tDCS polarity effects in motor and cognitive domains: a meta-analytical review. Exp Brain Res. 2012;216(1):1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Wiethoff S, Hamada M, Rothwell JC. Variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimulation. 2014;7(3):468–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Kwon YH, Jang SH. Onsite-effects of dual-hemisphere versus conventional single-hemisphere transcranial direct current stimulation: a functional MRI study. Neural Regen Res. 2012;7(24):1889–94.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Nozari N, Thompson-Schill SL. More attention when speaking: does it help or does it hurt? Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13):2770–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Lindenberg R, Nachtigall L, Meinzer M, Sieg MM, Floel A. Differential effects of dual and unihemispheric motor cortex stimulation in older adults. J Neurosci. 2013;33(21):9176–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Naros G, Geyer M, Koch S, Mayr L, Ellinger T, Grimm F, et al. Enhanced motor learning with bilateral transcranial direct current stimulation: impact of polarity or current flow direction? Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127(4):2119–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects’ non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103–10.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Fujimoto S, Yamaguchi T, Otaka Y, Kondo K, Tanaka S. Dual-hemisphere transcranial direct current stimulation improves performance in a tactile spatial discrimination task. Clin Neurophysiol. 2014;125(8):1669–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Mancuso LE, Ilieva IP, Hamilton RH, Farah MJ. Does transcranial direct current stimulation improve healthy working memory?: a meta-analytic review. J Cogn Neurosci. 2016:1–27. [Epub ahead of print].

  62. Hill AT, Fitzgerald PB, Hoy KE. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory: a systematic review and meta-analysis of findings from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimul. 2016;9(2):197–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. D'Esposito M, Postle BR, Ballard D, Lease J. Maintenance versus manipulation of information held in working memory: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Cognition. 1999;41(1):66–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Glahn DC, Kim J, Cohen MS, Poutanen VP, Therman S, Bava S, et al. Maintenance and manipulation in spatial working memory: dissociations in the prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2002;17(1):201–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL.: K-BIT: Kaufman brief intelligence test: American Guidance Service; 1990.

  66. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Wechsler D. Wechsler memory scale-III: administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Schretlen D. Brief Test of Attention professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Hummel F, Celnik P, Giraux P, Floel A, Wu WH, Gerloff C, et al. Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain. 2005;128(Pt 3):490–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Poreisz C, Boros K, Antal A, Paulus W. Safety aspects of transcranial direct current stimulation concerning healthy subjects and patients. Brain Res Bull. 2007;72(4-6):208–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Kessler SK, Turkeltaub PE, Benson JG, Hamilton RH. Differences in the experience of active and sham transcranial direct current stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2012;5(2):155–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. O'Connell NE, Cossar J, Marston L, Wand BM, Bunce D, Moseley GL, et al. Rethinking clinical trials of transcranial direct current stimulation: participant and assessor blinding is inadequate at intensities of 2mA. PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47514.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. van Steenburgh JJ, Varvaris M., Vannorsdall TD, Gordon B, Schretlen D.: Transcranial direct current stimulation during n-back strengthens default mode network integrity and enhances anticorrelation with task-positive networks. In: American Neurological Association 2014 Annual Meeting: 2014; Baltimore; 2014: #S118.

  74. Sandry J, DeLuca J, Chiaravalloti N. Working memory capacity links cognitive reserve with long-term memory in moderate to severe TBI: a translational approach. J Neurol. 2015;262(1):59–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, Wearing H. The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Dev Psychol. 2004;40(2):177–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Salthouse TA. Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing. Psychol Sci. 1991;2(3):179–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Arvanitakis Z, Bienias JL, Bennett DA. Subcortical infarcts, Alzheimer’s disease pathology, and memory function in older persons. Ann Neurol. 2007;62(1):59–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Dedoncker JB. Andre R.; Baeken, Chris; Vanderhasselt, Marie-Anne. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in healthy and neuropsychiatric samples: influence of stimulation parameters. Brain Stimulation. 2016;9(4):501–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Martin DM, Liu R, Alonzo A, Green M, Player MJ, Sachdev P, et al. Can transcranial direct current stimulation enhance outcomes from cognitive training? A randomized controlled trial in healthy participants. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;16(9):1927–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Park SH, Seo JH, Kim YH, Ko MH. Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. Neuroreport. 2014;25(2):122–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Tadini L, El-Nazer R, Brunoni AR, Williams J, Carvas M, Boggio P, et al. Cognitive, mood, and electroencephalographic effects of noninvasive cortical stimulation with weak electrical currents. J ECT. 2011;27(2):134–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Harty S, Robertson IH, Miniussi C, Sheehy OC, Devine CA, McCreery S, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation over right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances error awareness in older age. J Neurosci. 2014;34(10):3646–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Vannorsdall TD, Schretlen DJ, Andrejczuk M, Ledoux K, Bosley LV, Weaver JR, et al. Altering automatic verbal processes with transcranial direct current stimulation. Front Psychiatry. 2012;3:73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Holland R, Leff AP, Josephs O, Galea JM, Desikan M, Price CJ, et al. Speech facilitation by left inferior frontal cortex stimulation. Curr Biol. 2011;21(16):1403–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Gill J, Shah-Basak PP, Hamilton R. It’s the thought that counts: examining the task-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function. Brain Stimulation. 2015;8(2):253–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Meinzer M, Antonenko D, Lindenberg R, Hetzer S, Ulm L, Avirame K, et al. Electrical brain stimulation improves cognitive performance by modulating functional connectivity and task-specific activation. J Neurosci. 2012;32(5):1859–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Jacobson L, Ezra A, Berger U, Lavidor M. Modulating oscillatory brain activity correlates of behavioral inhibition using transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;123(5):979–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Wirth M, Rahman RA, Kuenecke J, Koenig T, Horn H, Sommer W, et al. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on behaviour and electrophysiology of language production. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(14):3989–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Stagg CJ, Lin RL, Mezue M, Segerdahl A, Kong Y, Xie J, et al. Widespread modulation of cerebral perfusion induced during and after transcranial direct current stimulation applied to the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2013;33(28):11425–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Polania R, Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Modulating functional connectivity patterns and topological functional organization of the human brain with transcranial direct current stimulation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(8):1236–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Chib VS, Yun K, Takahashi H, Shimojo S. Noninvasive remote activation of the ventral midbrain by transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e268–77.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  92. Lang N, Siebner HR, Ward NS, Lee L, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, et al. How does transcranial DC stimulation of the primary motor cortex alter regional neuronal activity in the human brain? Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22(2):495–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Kane MJ, Engle RW. The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev. 2002;9(4):637–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Park SH, Koh EJ, Choi HY, Ko MH. A double-blind, sham-controlled, pilot study to assess the effects of the concomitant use of transcranial direct current stimulation with the computer assisted cognitive rehabilitation to the prefrontal cortex on cognitive functions in patients with stroke. J Korean Neurosurg Soc. 2013;54(6):484–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Corbett BA, Constantine LJ. Autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: assessing attention and response control with the integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12(4-5):335–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Ochsner KN, Phelps E. Emerging perspectives on emotion-cognition interactions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(8):317–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Feeser M, Prehn K, Kazzer P, Mungee A, Bajbouj M. Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances cognitive control during emotion regulation. Brain Stimul. 2014;7(1):105–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Bogdanov M, Schwabe L. Transcranial stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex prevents stress-induced working memory deficits. J Neurosci. 2016;36(4):1429–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. D'Esposito M, Postle BR. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:115–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. Wu YJ, Tseng P, Chang CF, Pai MC, Hsu KS, Lin CC, et al. Modulating the interference effect on spatial working memory by applying transcranial direct current stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Cognition. 2014;91:87–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Nelson JT, McKinley RA, Golob EJ, Warm JS, Parasuraman R. Enhancing vigilance in operators with prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). NeuroImage. 2014;85(Pt 3):909–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Weber MJ, Messing SB, Rao H, Detre JA, Thompson-Schill SL. Prefrontal transcranial direct current stimulation alters activation and connectivity in cortical and subcortical reward systems: a tDCS-fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(8):3673–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Parkin BL, Ekhtiari H, Walsh VF. Non-invasive human brain stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a primer. Neuron. 2015;87(5):932–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Koshino H, Carpenter PA, Minshew NJ, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Just MA. Functional connectivity in an fMRI working memory task in high-functioning autism. NeuroImage. 2005;24(3):810–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Urbain C, Vogan VM, Ye AX, Pang EW, Doesburg SM, Taylor MJ. Desynchronization of fronto-temporal networks during working memory processing in autism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37(1):153–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Au J, Katz B, Buschkuehl M, Bunarjo K, Senger T, Zabel C, et al. Enhancing working memory training with transcranial direct current stimulation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2016;28(9):1419–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Kwon YH, Nam KS, Lee MY, Park JW, Jang SH. The temporal change of cortical activation induced by the ongoing effects of transcranial direct current stimulation. J Phys Ther Sci. 2011;23(1):65–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Shekhawat GS, Stinear CM, Searchfield GD. Transcranial direct current stimulation intensity and duration effects on tinnitus suppression. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(2):164–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Fricke K, Seeber AA, Thirugnanasambandam N, Paulus W, Nitsche MA, Rothwell JC. Time course of the induction of homeostatic plasticity generated by repeated transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105(3):1141–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, et al. Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(5):1590–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  111. Meinzer M, Jaehnigen S, Copland DA, Darkow R, Grittner U, Avirame K, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation over multiple days improves learning and maintenance of a novel vocabulary. Cortex. 2014;50:137–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Oberman L, Ifert-Miller F, Najib U, Bashir S, Woollacott I, Gonzalez-Heydrich J, et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation provides means to assess cortical plasticity and excitability in humans with fragile X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in synaptic neuroscience. 2010;2:26.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  113. Oberman LM, Ifert-Miller F, Najib U, Bashir S, Heydrich JG, Picker J, et al. Abnormal mechanisms of plasticity and metaplasticity in autism spectrum disorders and fragile X syndrome. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2016;26(7):617–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Giglia G, Brighina F, Rizzo S, Puma A, Indovino S, Maccora S, et al. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances memory-guided responses in a visuospatial working memory task. Funct Neurol. 2014;29(3):189–93.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Clark VP, Coffman BA, Mayer AR, Weisend MP, Lane TD, Calhoun VD, et al. TDCS guided using fMRI significantly accelerates learning to identify concealed objects. NeuroImage. 2012;59(1):117–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Teo F, Hoy KE, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzgerald PB. Investigating the role of current strength in tDCS modulation of working memory performance in healthy controls. Front Psychiatry. 2011;2(45.10):3389–95.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Jones KT, Stephens JA, Alam M, Bikson M, Berryhill ME. Longitudinal neurostimulation in older adults improves working memory. PLoS One. 2015;10(4):e0121904.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Patrick Chambers and Jessica O’Grady for help recruiting and testing study participants and Nancy Grund for editorial assistance. Portions of these data were presented as a poster at the 2015 Cognitive Neuroscience Society meeting and for oral presentations at the 2015 International Neuropsychology Society meeting and the 2016 American Academy of Neurology annual meeting.

All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Therapeutic Cognitive Neuroscience Fund (BG), the Therapeutic Cognitive Neuroscience Endowment (BG), and the Adith and Benjamin Miller Endowment for Autism, Aging, and Alzheimer’s Disease (BG). The funding sources provided salary support and paid for miscellaneous expenses associated with conducting the research described in the article.

Availability of data and materials

The data will be made publicly available.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JJvS designed and performed the research, analyzed data, and wrote the paper. MV designed and performed the research. DS designed the research and wrote the paper. TV designed the research. BG wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Jason van Steenburgh.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Institutional Review Board of The Johns Hopkins School of Medicine approved the study, and all participants gave written informed consent to participate.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Under an agreement with Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc., Dr. Schretlen is entitled to a share of royalty on sales of a test used in the study described in this article. The terms of this arrangement are being managed by the Johns Hopkins University in accordance with its conflict of interest policies. The other remaining authors declare that they have no conflict of interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

van Steenburgh, J.J., Varvaris, M., Schretlen, D.J. et al. Balanced bifrontal transcranial direct current stimulation enhances working memory in adults with high-functioning autism: a sham-controlled crossover study. Molecular Autism 8, 40 (2017). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13229-017-0152-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13229-017-0152-x

Keywords