Skip to main content

Reduced isolation-induced pup ultrasonic communication in mouse pups lacking brain serotonin

Abstract

Background

Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) is a key modulatory neurotransmitter in the mammalian central nervous system (CNS) that plays an important role as a developmental signal. Several lines of evidence associate altered 5-HT signaling with psychopathology in humans, particularly neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorders (ASD). ASD are characterized by persistent social and communication deficits along with stereotyped and repetitive patterns of behavior, with all symptoms emerging early during development.

Methods

Here, we employed a mouse model devoid of brain 5-HT due to the lack of the gene encoding tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (Tph2), the initial and rate-limiting enzyme of 5-HT synthesis in the CNS. Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice show normal prenatal development; however, they display for yet unknown reasons severe growth retardation during the first postnatal weeks. We investigated, therefore, whether Tph2 -/- mice display deficits in isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) as pups during early life. Isolation-induced USV are the most commonly studied behavioral measure to assess developmental delays and communication deficits in rodent models for ASD, particularly as they serve an important communicative function in coordinating mother-pup interactions.

Results

Tph2 -/- mouse pups displayed a clear deficit in the emission of isolation-induced USV, as compared to heterozygous and wildtype littermates, exactly during growth retardation onset, including reduced call numbers and deficits in call clustering and temporal organization.

Conclusions

The ultrasonic communication impairment displayed by Tph2 -/- mouse pups is likely to result in a deficient mother-infant interaction, presumably contributing to their growth retardation phenotype, and represents a prominent feature relevant to ASD.

Background

Serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) is a key modulatory neurotransmitter in the mammalian central nervous system (CNS; [1]). The 5-HT system is one of the earliest developing neurotransmitter systems, with 5-HT itself and expression of associated enzymes and receptors all reaching peak levels during early brain development before declining to adult levels, reflecting its important role as a developmental signal [2,3]. It is, therefore, not surprising that several lines of evidence associate altered 5-HT signaling with psychopathology in humans, particularly neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorders (ASD; [4]).

ASD are characterized by persistent social and communication deficits along with stereotyped and repetitive patterns of behavior, with all symptoms emerging early during development [5]. In rodent models for ASD, however, behavioral phenotypes with relevance to the diagnostic criteria are typically assessed in adulthood, since few behavioral test paradigms are available that allow the reliable assessment of such complex behavioral alterations in infancy [6,7]. Yet, a remarkable exception appears to be the experimentally controlled induction of ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) in infant rodents during isolation from mother and littermates for a short period of time. Such isolation-induced USV are commonly studied to assess developmental delays and communication deficits in rodent models for ASD, with model organisms typically displaying overall reduced USV levels or an ontogenetic shift in the inverted U-shaped developmental call emission pattern [8-10].

The involvement of the 5-HT system in isolation-induced pup USV has been extensively investigated in pharmacological studies. Diminishing central 5-HT levels by means of the neurotoxin 5,7 dihydroxytryptamine (5,7-DHT) or the inhibitor of the 5-HT synthesizing enzyme tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH), para-chlorophenylalanine (PCPA), led to a strong reduction in USV emission, with PCPA effects being dose-dependently antagonized by the 5-HT precursor 5-hydroxytryptophan (5-HTP; [11]). Consistently, the long-term reduction in 5-HT content following repeated MDMA treatment was also found to reduce USV emission rates [12]. Furthermore, inhibiting 5-HT function by the application of 8-OH-DPAT, which works mostly through activation of the 5-HT1A autoreceptor [13], led to an inconsistent result pattern in mouse pups [14,15], but dose-dependently decreased isolation-induced USV in rat pups in a comparatively consistent manner [12,16-23]. However, not only decreasing 5-HT function, but also acutely increasing extracellular 5-HT levels by selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) reduces isolation-induced USV in mice [24] and rats [20,21,25-29]. Likewise, 5-HT release induced by acute MDMA treatment also results in the inhibition of isolation-induced USV [12].

While the pharmacological approach strongly suggests the involvement of 5-HT in isolation-induced USV, this approach has a number of limitations, possibly accounting for at least some of the obtained inconsistencies: (1) 5,7-DHT treatment leads to the lesion of 5-HTergic neurons, also affecting 5-HT cotransmitter levels; thus, observed effects might not exclusively be due to the lack of 5-HT per se; (2) PCPA does not only inhibit the synthesis of central 5-HT, but also alters its synthesis in the periphery; (3) both 5,7-DHT and PCPA cause only a partial decrease in 5-HT content; (4) 5-HT receptor agonists/antagonists are typically not selective for a specific receptor subtype and might also activate/inhibit non-5-HT receptors; finally, (5) at least some of the observed effects on isolation-induced USV following the administration of 5-HT receptor ligands might be unspecific and actually be caused by their sedative or thermoregulatory actions [9].

In the present study, we aimed to overcome the limitations of the pharmacological approach in studying the impact of brain 5-HT on isolation-induced USV by means of a genetic mouse model lacking the central form of TPH, TPH2 [30], and, thus, genetically depleted of central 5-HT [31]. Previous studies showed that Tph2 null mutant mice not only exhibit severe growth retardation during early development [31-35], but also a number of behavioral alterations in adulthood, most notably in the emotional domain, being characterized by decreased anxiety-related behavior, but enhanced impulsivity [36,37]. In addition, very prominent deficits were seen in the social domain, with a lack of maternal care [31,38], strongly increased aggression [31,34,36,37,39], and deficits in sexual behavior [40,41]. In fact, a recent study suggested that Tph2 null mutant mice display deficits with relevance to all ASD core symptoms, including sociability, scent marking, and repetitive behavior, while acoustic communication was not assessed [33]. Here, we investigated, therefore, whether Tph2 null mutant mouse pups display deficits in isolation-induced USV, the most commonly studied behavioral measure to assess developmental delays and communication deficits in rodent models for ASD.

Methods

Ethics approval

All procedures were conducted in strict compliance with the National Institutes of Health Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the legal requirements of Germany. Procedures were approved by the ethical committee of the local government (Regierungspräsidium, Gießen, Germany).

Animals and housing

Isolation-induced pup USV were assessed in Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice with a targeted deletion of exon 1 and 2 in the Tph2 gene and compared to Tph2 heterozygous (Tph2 +/-) and Tph2 wildtype (Tph2 +/+) mice. Mice were obtained from mutant lines originally generated by Alenina et al. [31] and backcrossed to C57BL/6N (Charles River, Sulzfeld, Germany; F10 generation; [37]). Using a heterozygous breeding protocol, Tph2 +/- males and females were paired for breeding in a conventional vivarium at the Biomedical Research Center of the Philipps-University of Marburg, Germany. Approximately 2 weeks after pairing for breeding, females were individually housed and inspected daily for pregnancy and delivery. The day of birth was considered as postnatal day (PND) 0. In total, pups from seven litters were included in the experiment. In all litters included, all three genotypes were present, namely Tph2 -/-, Tph2 +/-, and Tph2 +/+ littermates. Bedding and a wooden board were provided in each cage. Standard rodent chow and water were available ad libitum. The colony room was maintained on a 12:12 light/dark cycle with lights on at 06:00 h, at approximately 22°C and 40% to 50% humidity. Pups were identified by paw tattoo, using non-toxic animal tattoo ink (Ketchum permanent Tattoo Inks green paste, Ketchum Manufacturing Inc., Brockville, Canada). The ink was inserted subcutaneously through a 30-gauge hypodermic needle tip into the center of the paw, as in previous studies [42,43].

Genotyping

Mouse tail snips were collected by dissecting approximately 0.3 cm of tail. Tails were digested in a buffer containing 0.2 SDS and 1 mg/ml proteinase K, diluted in TE buffer containing 20 μg/ml RNase A and subsequently used as a DNA template for PCR reaction with the following primers: TPH34 (5′-AGC TGA GGC AGA CAG AAA GG-3′), TPH54 (5′-CCA AAG AGC TAC TCG ACC TAC G-3′), and Neo3 (5′-CTG CGC TGA CAG CCG GAA CAC-3′).

Behavioral testing

To study developmental aspects in isolation-induced pup USV in a genotype- and sex-dependent manner, an experimental design with three independent variables was used, namely genotype, sex, and development. To this aim, male and female Tph2 -/-, Tph2 +/-, and Tph2 +/+ littermates were repeatedly tested on PND3, PND6, and PND9. Recordings of isolation-induced USV were conducted during the light phase of the 12:12 h light/dark cycle. After completion of behavioral experiments on PND3, pups were tattooed for their identification and tail samples were taken for genotyping. Experimenters were blind to genotypes during data acquisition and analysis.

Isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations - recording

To induce isolation-induced USV, pups were isolated from mother and littermates for 10 min under room temperature (20°C to 23°C). Pups were individually removed from the nest at random and gently placed into an isolation container (10 × 8 × 6 cm; open surface) made of glass, containing fresh bedding material. The isolation container was surrounded by a sound attenuating box (21 × 21 × 21 cm) made of Styrofoam (thickness of walls: 6 cm). USV emission was monitored by an UltraSoundGate Condenser Microphone CM 16 (Avisoft Bioacoustics, Berlin, Germany) placed in the roof of the sound attenuating box, 22 cm above the floor. The microphone was connected via an UltraSoundGate 416 USGH audio device (Avisoft Bioacoustics) to a personal computer, where acoustic data were recorded with a sampling rate of 250,000 Hz in 16-bit format by Avisoft RECORDER (version 2.97; Avisoft Bioacoustics). The microphone that was used for recording was sensitive to frequencies of 15 to 180 kHz with a flat frequency response (±6 dB) between 25 and 140 kHz. Prior to each test, the behavioral equipment was cleaned using a 0.1 % acetic acid solution and dried with paper towels.

Isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations - analysis

For acoustical analysis, recordings were transferred to Avisoft SASLab Pro (version 5.20; Avisoft Bioacoustics), and a fast Fourier transform was conducted (512 FFT length, 100% frame, Hamming window, and 75% time window overlap). Correspondingly, the spectrograms were produced at 488 Hz of frequency resolution and 0.512 ms of time resolution. Call detection was provided by an automatic threshold-based algorithm (amplitude threshold: −40 dB) and a hold-time mechanism (hold time: 10 ms). Since no USV were detected below 30 kHz, a high-pass filter of 30 kHz was used to reduce background noise outside the relevant frequency band to 0 dB. The accuracy of call detection by the software was verified manually by an experienced user. When necessary, missed calls were marked by hand to be included in the automatic parameter analysis by trained experimenters. The total number of USV was calculated for the entire 10-min test session. Based on previous studies of isolation-induced pup USV [42-44], latency to start calling, call duration, peak frequency, peak amplitude, and frequency modulation were also included (for details, see [42]). In addition, call subtypes were determined by means of density blots depicting peak frequency versus peak amplitude, as described before [10]. Finally, to assess the temporal organization of isolation-induced USV emission, sequential analyses were performed by correlating the durations of given isolation-induced USV with the durations of the previous ones (N − 1), the ones two before (N − 2), and the ones three before (N − 3), as described before [10].

Developmental milestones and somatosensory reflexes

In addition to isolation-induced USV, developmental milestones and somatosensory reflexes were assessed by trained experimenters on PND3, PND6, and PND9. After the 10-min isolation period, body weight, body temperature, surface righting, and vertical screen holding were determined as described in Wöhr et al. [42], with a maximum latency of 30 s for surface righting and vertical screen holding. Body weight was measured using a palmscale (PS6-250; My Weigh Europe, Hückelhoven, Germany). For body temperature determination, a Testo 110 thermometer with surface sensor (Testo AG, Lenzkirch, Germany) was used. Body temperature was measured by gentle application of the thermal probe onto the stomach of the mouse pup for 20 s.

Statistical analysis

For analysis of isolation-induced USV, developmental milestones, and somatosensory reflexes, ANOVAs for repeated measurements with the between-subjects factors genotype and sex, and the within-subject factor development were calculated. Litter was used as covariate. Approximately half of the Tph2 +/- mice were randomly excluded from the experiment to obtain similar numbers of mice per genotype in order to improve the quality of statistical comparisons, resulting in the following group sizes: 10 Tph2 -/-, 11 Tph2 +/-, and 12 Tph2 +/+ littermates. ANOVAs were followed by LSD post hoc analysis when appropriate. A P value of < .050 was considered statistically significant. The covariate litter never reached statistical significance (all P values > .050).

Results

To evaluate whether isolation-induced pup USV are altered in the absence of brain 5-HT in mice, we evaluated USV at three developmental stages, namely PND3, PND6, and PND9, and analyzed developmental, genotype, and sex effects in the USV emission pattern of Tph2 -/- , Tph2 +/-, and Tph2 +/+ littermates.

Isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations - development

Emission rates of isolation-induced USV changed with development (main effect development: F 2,52 = 13.916, P < .001; Figure 1A), when including all genotypes in the analysis. Specifically, an inverted U-shaped developmental call emission pattern was detected, with call emission rates peaking on PND6 but comparatively low levels of isolation-induced USV on PND3 and PND9. Call duration also changed with development (main effect development: F 2,42 = 3.428, P = .042; Figure 1B). Yet, its developmental profile markedly differed from call number as it was characterized by a gradual decrease, with particularly short isolation-induced USV on PND9. Latency to start calling, peak frequency, peak amplitude, and frequency modulation did not change with development (all P values > .050; Figure 1C,D).

Figure 1
figure 1

Isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) in Tph2 -/- mouse pups emitted at postnatal days (PND) 3, 6, and 9. (A) Total number (n), (B) average call duration in milliseconds (ms), (C) average call peak frequency in kilohertz (kHz), and (D) average call peak amplitude in decibel (dB) of isolation-induced USV emitted during the 10-min isolation from mother and littermates. Black circles: Tph2 wildtype (Tph2 +/+) control mice; grey circles: Tph2 heterozygous (Tph2 +/-) mice; white circles: Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice. Data are presented as means ± standard errors of the mean. *P < .050 Tph2 +/+ vs. Tph2 -/-.

Isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations - genotype

The developmental emission pattern of isolation-induced USV differed between genotypes (main effect genotype: F 2,26 = 2.700, P = .086; interaction genotype × development: F 4,52 = 3.526, P = .013; Figure 1A). On PND3, Tph2 -/- mouse pups emitted fewer isolation-induced USV than Tph2 +/+ controls (P = .016) and tended to emit fewer than Tph2 +/- mouse pups (P = .069), with the latter not differing from each other (P = .517). Similarly, on PND6, again Tph2 -/- mouse pups emitted fewer isolation-induced USV than Tph2 +/+ controls (P = .024), but did not differ from Tph2 +/- mouse pups (P = .125), with the latter also not differing from each other (P = .434). On PND9, genotypes did not differ from each other (all P values > .050). Importantly, when including maximum one pup per genotype and sex per litter by randomly excluding the additional ones, a very similar result pattern was obtained. Again, the developmental emission pattern of isolation-induced USV differed between genotypes (main effect genotype: F 2,19 = 1.986, P = .165; interaction genotype × development: F 4,38 = 3.340, P = .021). On PND3 and PND6, Tph2 -/- mouse pups emitted fewer isolation-induced USV than Tph2 +/+ controls (P = .034 and P = .031; all other P values > .050), consistent with the statistical analysis including additional littermates. Also in line with it, on PND9, genotypes did not differ from each other (all P values > .050). As no genotype effects on the latency to start calling (main effect genotype: F 2,26 = .379, P = .689; interaction genotype × development: F 4,52 = .691, P = .601) and call duration (main effect genotype: F 2,21 = .339, P = .717; interaction genotype × development: F 4,42 = 2.204, P = .085; Figure 1B) were detected, this indicates that genotype primarily affected call repetition rate, i.e., numbers of isolation-induced USV per time interval. Peak frequency, peak amplitude, and frequency modulation were not affected by genotype (all P values > .050; Figure 1C,D).

Isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations - sex

Sex had only a minor effect on the emission of isolation-induced USV. Specifically, a sex difference was obtained for call duration, with females emitting calls with longer durations than males, irrespective of genotype (main effect sex: F 1,21 = 5.773, P = .026; interaction sex × development: F 2,42 = 3.058, P = .058; interaction genotype × sex: F 2,21 = 2.701, P = .090; interaction genotype × sex × development: F 4,42 = 2.221, P = .083). Importantly, while in males a gradual decrease in call duration with age was observed, call duration was highest on PND6 in females, reflecting an inverted U-shaped developmental pattern. No evidence for sex differences was obtained for the latency to start calling, call number, peak frequency, peak amplitude, and frequency modulation (all P values > .050).

Isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations - detailed analyses

A more detailed analysis was performed to identify clusters of isolation-induced USV emitted by Tph2 -/- mouse pups and Tph2 +/+ controls by means of density plots (Figure 2A,B). For this subsequent analysis, isolation-induced USV emitted on PND6 were used, as an inverted U-shaped developmental call emission pattern was detected in the first step, with call emission rates peaking on PND6, but comparatively low levels of isolation-induced USV on PND3 and PND9. In Tph2 +/+ controls, two clusters were identified, with most isolation-induced USV either being characterized by peak frequencies between 60 to 70 and 80 to 110 kHz. Two very similar clusters were obtained in Tph2 -/- mouse pups. However, the two clusters were not as clearly segregated as in Tph2 +/+ controls, and the distribution of isolation-induced USV was affected by genotype. Furthermore, in Tph2 -/- mouse pups, the most common peak frequency was below 60 kHz and therefore clearly lower than the most common peak frequency in Tph2 +/+ controls, as also evident from peak frequency histograms (Figure 2C). In fact, when comparing peak frequencies between Tph2 -/- mouse pups and Tph2 +/+ controls on PND6 only, genotypes differed from each other (t 20 = 2.275, P = .034). Also, a substantial number of isolation-induced USV emitted by Tph2 -/- mouse pups were characterized by comparatively low peak amplitudes (Figure 2D), resulting in a trend for louder isolation-induced USV in Tph2 +/+ controls, as compared to Tph2 -/- mouse pups on PND6 (t 20 = 1.968, P = .063). An additional sequential analysis of the durations of subsequent isolation-induced USV finally indicated that the call emission pattern is not random in Tph2 +/+ controls, since the durations of given isolation-induced USV could be predicted by the durations of the previous ones (N − 1) and by the ones two before by trend (N − 2), but not by the ones three before (N − 3). Evidence for such a non-random call emission pattern was also obtained in Tph2 -/- mouse pups but in fewer cases. While substantial correlations of r > 0.200 between given isolation-induced USV and the durations of the previous ones (N − 1) were found in 100% of the Tph2 +/+ controls, such correlations were found in only 80% of the Tph2 -/- mouse pups. Furthermore, correlation coefficients were found to be lower in Tph2 -/- mouse pups, as compared to Tph2 +/+ controls (N − 1: t 20 = 3.146, P = .005; N − 2: t 20 = 2.025, P = .063; N − 3: t 20 = 1.698, P = .105; Figure 3).

Figure 2
figure 2

Distribution of individual isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) in Tph2 -/- mouse pups emitted at postnatal day (PND) 6. Density plots depicting the distribution of individual isolation-induced USV depending on call peak frequency in kilohertz (kHz) and call peak amplitude in decibel (dB) in Tph2 wildtype (Tph2 +/+) control mice (A) and Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice (B), with color coding reflecting frequencies as percentages. Frequency histograms depicting the distribution of individual USV depending on call peak frequency in kilohertz (kHz) (C) and call peak amplitude in decibel (dB) (D) in percentages, with isolation-induced USV emitted by Tph2 wildtype (Tph2 +/+) control mice (black area) and Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice (grey area).

Figure 3
figure 3

Sequential analysis of the durations of subsequent isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations (USV) indicating a non-random call emission pattern in Tph2 -/- mouse pups at postnatal day (PND) 6. Correlations between the call durations of given isolation-induced USV and the call durations of the previous ones (N − 1), the call durations of the ones two before (N − 2), or the call durations of the ones three before (N − 3) for Tph2 wildtype (Tph2 +/+) control mice (black circles) and Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice (white circles). *P < .100 Tph2 +/+ vs. Tph2 -/-; **P < .005 Tph2 +/+ vs. Tph2 -/-.

Developmental milestones and somatosensory reflexes - development

As expected, all developmental milestones, namely body temperature regulation (F 2,52 = 9.220, P < .001; Figure 4A) and body weight gain (F 2,52 = 444.921, P < .001; Figure 4B), as well as one of the two somatosensory reflexes assessed, namely surface righting (F 2,50 = 25.517, P < .001), but not vertical screen holding (F 2,50 = 1.779, P = .179), varied with age. Body temperature and body weight increased with age, whereas the time needed for surface righting decreased with age (data not shown in detail).

Figure 4
figure 4

Developmental profile of Tph2 -/- mouse pups at postnatal days (PND) 3, 6, and 9. (A) Body temperature in degrees Celsius (°C) and (B) body weight in grams (g) in pups tested for isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations. Black circles: Tph2 wildtype (Tph2 +/+) control mice; grey circles: Tph2 heterozygous (Tph2 +/-) mice; white circles: Tph2 null mutant (Tph2 -/-) mice. Data are presented as means ± standard errors of the mean. **P < .005 Tph2 +/+ vs. Tph2 -/-; ***P < .001 Tph2 +/+ vs. Tph2 -/-; ## P < .005 Tph2 +/- vs. Tph2 -/-; ### P < .001 Tph2 +/- vs. Tph2 -/-. Please note that the error bars in B are too small to be visible.

Developmental milestones and somatosensory reflexes - genotype

Body temperature regulation differed between genotypes (main effect genotype: F 2,26 = 13.415, P < .001; interaction genotype × development: F 4,52 = 3.731, P = .010; Figure 4A). Differences in body temperature emerged with development. While genotypes did not differ on PND3 (all P values > .050), prominent differences were detected on PND6 and PND9. On both days, Tph2 -/- mouse pups had lower body temperatures than Tph2 +/- (P = .007 and P = .002) and Tph2 +/+ (P = .001 and P < .001) mouse pups, with the latter not differing from each other (P = .384 and P = .595). Even more prominent genotype differences were obtained for body weight gain (main effect genotype: F 2,26 = 113.319, P < .001; interaction genotype × development: F 4,52 = 153.783, P < .001; Figure 4B). From PND3 onward, Tph2 -/- mouse pups had lower body weights than Tph2 +/- (P < .001, P < .001, and P < .001) and Tph2 +/+ (P < .001, P < .001, and P < .001) mouse pups, with the latter not differing from each other (all P values > .050). Genotypes did not differ in the two somatosensory reflexes assessed, namely surface righting and vertical screen holding (all P values > .050).

Developmental milestones and somatosensory reflexes - sex

Body temperature regulation differed between sexes in a genotype-dependent manner (main effect sex: F 1,26 = .750, P = .394; interaction sex × development: F 2,52 = .902, P = .405; interaction genotype × sex: F 2,26 = .544, P = .587; interaction genotype × sex × development: F 4,52 = 4.380, P = .004). In females, no genotype differences were obtained on PND3 (all P values > .050). However, while in female Tph2 +/- and Tph2 +/+ mouse pups, body temperature increased with development, no such increase was evident in female Tph2 -/- mouse pups. This led to genotype differences on PND6 and PND9. On both days, female Tph2 -/- mouse pups had lower body temperatures than Tph2 +/+ controls (P = .025 and P = .048), but not Tph2 +/- mouse pups (P = .290 and P = .067), with the latter not differing from each other (P = .173 and P = .852). Conversely, in males, body temperatures increased in all three genotypes, but already on PND3, Tph2 -/- mouse pups had body temperatures lower than the ones of Tph2 +/- and Tph2 +/+ mouse pups (P = .001 and P = .028), with the latter not differing from each other (P = .052). On PND6, genotypes differed again, with Tph2 -/- mouse pups having lower body temperatures than Tph2 +/- and Tph2 +/+ mouse pups (P = .011 and P = .010), with the latter not differing from each other (P = .934). On PND9, no genotype differences were detected (all P values > .050). Sex had no effect on body weight gain, surface righting, and vertical screen holding (all P values > .050).

Correlations between isolation-induced pup ultrasonic vocalizations and developmental milestones

No significant correlations between isolation-induced USV and body weight or body temperature were detected, neither on PND3, PND6, nor PND9 (all P values > .100), with only one exception (isolation-induced USV emission rate and body temperature on PND3 in Tph2+/- mouse pups: r = .715 and P = .013).

Discussion

In this study, we investigated for the first time whether Tph2 -/- mouse pups display deficits in isolation-induced USV, a widely used behavioral measure to assess developmental delays and communication deficits in rodent models for ASD and linked it to the growth retardation phenotype reported before [31-35]. Consistent with previous studies, very prominent genotype differences were obtained for body weight gain. From PND3 onward, Tph2 -/- mouse pups had clearly lower body weights than Tph2 +/- and Tph2 +/+ mouse pups. Delayed growth was accompanied by lower body temperatures in Tph2 -/- mouse pups, possibly reflecting a deficit in thermoregulation. While genotypes did not differ on PND3, prominent differences were detected on PND6 and PND9, with body temperatures being relatively low overall due to the 10-min isolation from dam and littermates. On both days, Tph2 -/- mouse pups had lower body temperatures than Tph2 +/- and Tph2 +/+ mouse pups, consistent with our previous reports on deficits in body temperature regulation in adult Tph2 -/- mice [31]. Despite the clear effects on body weight gain and body temperature regulation, the two somatosensory reflexes assessed, namely surface righting and vertical screen holding, were not affected by genotype. This is in line with a recent study by Kane et al. [33] not reporting any differences in surface righting and forepaw grasping, but in other somatosensory reflexes not related to the ones determined in the present study, such as air righting and negative geotaxis. The latter is consistent with the fact that 5-HT is important for the modulation of early motor responses [45], and a more detailed assessment of the development of early motor functions in Tph2 -/- mouse pups appears therefore to be a promising future study.

So far, the reasons for the delayed growth in Tph2 -/- mice remain unknown. We reported that impaired thermoregulation and a dysfunction of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis are not the primary causes of the growth retardation [35]. Also, dys-/hypophagia could be ruled out as Tph2 -/- mouse pups were reported to display normal suckling activity and had filled milk pouches [35], despite 5-HT being implicated in the regulation of the early suckling response [46]. Interestingly, during prenatal development, we did not see differences in body weight in Tph2 -/- mice and Tph2 +/+ controls [35], indicating that the developmental delay emerges after birth. Starting from about PND3, Tph2 -/- mouse pups are smaller, have soft skin, and differ in body weight [31-35]. Here, we now show for the first time that Tph2 -/- mouse pups display a clear deficit in the emission of isolation-induced USV, exactly during growth retardation onset. On both PND3 and PND6, Tph2 -/- mouse pups emitted fewer isolation-induced USV than Tph2 +/+ controls. The inverted U-shaped developmental call emission pattern that is typically seen in mouse pups is clearly detectable in Tph2 +/+ controls but less prominent in Tph2 -/- mouse pups. The absence of genotype effects on the latency to start calling and call duration indicates that the lack of brain 5-HT primarily affected call repetition rate, i.e., numbers of isolation-induced USV per time interval. While an overall quantitative comparison of the averages for peak frequency, peak amplitude, and frequency modulation did not reveal additional genotype differences, a recently developed more detailed analysis using density plots for individual isolation-induced USV [10] helped to detect 5-HT effects on call clustering on PND6. Confirming the results of the detailed analysis [10] of data obtained in a previous study [42], two clusters were identified in Tph2 +/+ controls, with most isolation-induced USV either being characterized by peak frequencies between 60 to 70 and 80 to 110 kHz. In Tph2 -/- mouse pups, two similar clusters were obtained, yet they were not as clearly segregated as in the Tph2 +/+ controls. Also, the most common peak frequency was below 60 kHz and therefore clearly lower than in the Tph2 +/+ controls. A substantial number of isolation-induced USV emitted by the Tph2 -/- mouse pups were further characterized by comparatively low peak amplitudes. Finally, an additional sequential analysis of the durations of subsequent isolation-induced USV indicated that the call emission pattern is not random in all Tph2 +/+ controls tested, since the durations of given isolation-induced USV could be predicted by the durations of the previous ones. Evidence for such a non-random call emission pattern was also obtained in Tph2 -/- mouse pups but in fewer cases. A similarly distorted sequential organization was recently reported the first time for a genetic mouse model for ASD, the Shank1 deficient mouse [10], which displays a variety of behavioral alterations with relevance to ASD [42,47-49] (for a call emission pattern analysis in Shank2 deficient mice, see [50]). Sex was found to have a minor effect on the emission of isolation-induced USV. Specifically, a sex difference was obtained for call duration but not call number, with females emitting calls with longer durations than males, irrespective of genotype. The comparatively minor effect of sex is consistent with the vast majority of the literature [51-54] (but see [55]). Interestingly, however, we observed longer call durations in females than in males in a number of our previous studies on pup isolation-induced USV, at least in wildtype mice from various strains, including C57BL/6J and 129S6/SvEvTac [42,44,56].

It is unlikely that the observed reduction in isolation-induced USV in Tph2 -/- mouse pups is simply due to the deficits in thermoregulation caused by a lack of brain 5-HT. This is because the developmental trajectories of the deficits in isolation-induced USV and thermoregulation do not match. For instance, a marked reduction in isolation-induced USV was already evident on PND3, at which differences in body temperature were not yet detected. Also, on PND9, when deficits in thermoregulation were most prominent, isolation-induced USV did not differ between genotypes. The same might be true for body weight. Again, most prominent genotype differences in body weight were evident on PND9, when no genotype differences in isolation-induced USV were detected. While deficits in thermoregulation and body weight gain are therefore unlikely to be responsible for the observed reduction in isolation-induced USV in Tph2 -/- mouse pups, the opposite might be true, namely that the reduced USV levels result in thermoregulatory deficits and delayed body weight gain. In rats, it was shown that surgical devocalization, resulting in a complete lack of isolation-induced USV, slowed rewarming of hypothermic pups [57], yet additional experiments are needed to test whether the reduction in USV emission displayed by Tph2 -/- mouse pups is sufficient to cause similar thermoregulatory deficits. In such future experiments, it would be ideal to assess changes in body temperature continuously during isolation from dam and littermates in order to link body temperature changes and the emission of isolation-induced USV over time, for instance, by using a fine rectal thermocouple, as done by Cummings et al. [58]. By means of the present approach, no robust correlations between the two measures were detected.

Isolation-induced USV in mouse pups serve an important communicative function in coordinating mother-pup interactions. Specifically, pup isolation-induced USV elicit maternal behavior, such as search and retrieval behavior, as repeatedly shown in playback experiments [44,59-63]. It was further shown that dams can distinguish between different USV types, and that they prefer certain types over others if given the choice, indicating that acoustic parameters, such as call duration, peak frequency, and peak amplitude, affect the communicative value of isolation-induced USV [44,60,61,63]. Because Tph2 -/- mouse pups emit not just fewer isolation-induced USV, but also USV that are characterized by a lower amplitude level, as compared to Tph2 +/+ controls, it appears likely that Tph2 -/- mouse pups are less efficient in attracting mothers and inducing maternal care. Impaired ultrasonic communication in Tph2 -/- mouse pups might therefore explain the growth retardation phenotype emerging around PND3. It is consistent with that view that Tph2 -/- mice slowly catch up in body weight during later developmental periods when the maternal caregiving behavior is becoming less crucial for the further development of the offspring [31,33-35].

Our finding of reduced isolation-induced USV in Tph2 -/- mouse pups lacking brain 5-HT is consistent with the view that the 5-HT system is strongly involved in the regulation of ultrasonic communication in pups. In particular, it is in line with the observation that the reduction of central 5-HT levels by means of various approaches, including the neurotoxin 5,7-DHT, the 5-HT synthesis inhibitor PCPA, and repeated MDMA treatment, blocks isolation-induced USV in rat pups [11,12]. Conversely, however, also increased 5-HT levels following acute MDMA and SSRI treatment, including fluoxetine, paroxetine, and zimeldine, were associated with reduced isolation-induced USV in mice [24] and rats [20,21,25-29]. Likewise, some tricyclic antidepressants inhibiting 5-HT reuptake, such as clomipramine, also inhibit pup isolation-induced USV, while for others, no or even opposite effects were reported [20,21,25-27,29,64-67]. Yet, all of these drugs have several off-target effects besides affecting the 5-HT transporter system in the CNS. Considering that all these pharmacological manipulations resulted only in a temporal and/or partial change in 5-HT content, but still profoundly altered isolation-induced USV emission, it might come as a surprise that a complete lack of brain 5-HT does not result in a complete lack of pup USV. Furthermore, it still needs to be elucidated which 5-HT receptors in particular modulate isolation-induced USV emission, despite comparative pharmacological studies targeting various 5-HT receptors [14-23] and some studies in genetic mouse models pointing towards the importance of 5-HT1A and 5-HT1B receptors in modulating isolation-induced USV [68-72].

Our present findings further fit nicely to known behavioral phenotypes of Tph2 -/- mice. For instance, they display clearly decreased levels of anxiety-related behavior in elevated plus maze, novelty suppressed feeding, and light-dark box [36,37]. Isolation-induced USV were repeatedly linked to anxiety, particularly in pharmacological studies, and it is believed that high call emission rates reflect high anxiety levels. For instance, benzodiazepines, such as diazepam and chlordiazepoxide, but also partial 5-HT1A agonists, including buspirone, consistently led to a dose-dependent decrease in isolation-induced USV [12,15,19-21,23,25-27,64-67,73]. Thus, the greater the amount of isolation-induced USV in pups, the greater the anxiety level observed in the mouse model. Our present findings clearly support this existing view and decreased calling after isolation in Tph2 -/- mouse pups might be correlated with reduced anxiety-related behavior in adulthood [36,37]. However, it has to be noted that the reduced level of anxiety-related behavior in elevated plus maze, novelty suppressed feeding, and light-dark box displayed by Tph2 -/- mice is possibly caused by enhanced impulsivity, as suggested by overall reduced response latencies [36,37]. Also, in the social context, increased impulsivity is evident. Tph2 -/- mice were found to display increased aggression, as reflected by both a decrease in the latency to attack intruders and an increase in the number of attacks, paralleled by increased testosterone levels [31,34,36,37,39].

However, such prominent deficits in the social domain are also consistent with the reduction in isolation-induced USV displayed by Tph2 -/- mouse pups. In fact, the obtained evidence for impairment in early mother-infant communication is in accordance with a recent study suggesting that Tph2 -/- mice display deficits with relevance to all ASD core symptoms [33]. In line with its role as a developmental signal, altered 5-HT signaling has been repeatedly associated with neurodevelopmental disorders, most notably ASD [4]. Importantly, in the CNS, 5-HT innervation seems to be transiently increased during infancy. Specifically, Chugani et al. [74,75] showed by positron emission tomography of a tryptophan analog that children with ASD do not display a peak in brain 5-HT synthesis during early development that is seen in healthy controls. Interestingly, this developmental alteration in 5-HT synthesis was found to be linked to deficits in language acquisition [76]. Also, more recently, decreased serotonin transporter (SERT) binding has been reported in children with ASD [77]. In line with these findings, alterations in genes encoding components of the 5-HT system have been repeatedly reported [4], including TPH2 [78-80] (but see [81-84]), and hence, a number of genetic mouse models for ASD targeted the 5-HT system, with isolation-induced USV being assessed in some of them. For instance, mice carrying a SERT ALA56 mutation, which was originally found in the human SERT gene of ASD families, display an increase in 5-HT levels [85] and recapitulate some features of ASD-like behavior, including social dysfunction and altered isolation-induced USV emission rates [85,86]. Likewise, SERT knockout mice were also found to display altered levels of isolation-induced USV [87]. Importantly, changes in 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptor activity were observed in the SERT ALA56 model [85]. As the 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptor are both involved in the regulation of isolation-induced USV, it is not clear whether the changes observed in the SERT ALA56 are due to altered 5-HT content. Furthermore, mice lacking Celf6, a gene identified by translational profiling of 5-HTergic neurons, were found to have lower 5-HT brain levels and to display fewer isolation-induced USV [88]. Finally, in the 15q11-13 duplication model for ASD, in which increased responses to 5-HT2C receptor signaling was observed, the rate of isolation-induced USV is clearly elevated [89]. Yet, in none of these studies was the specific effects of a decrease and/or lack of brain 5-HT on isolation-induced USV studied.

Conclusions

In summary, Tph2 -/- mouse pups displayed ultrasonic communication impairment during early development that is likely to result in a deficient mother-infant interaction, presumably contributing to their growth retardation phenotype. Moreover, the ultrasonic communication impairment is consistent with other behavioral phenotypes displayed by Tph2 -/- mice and represents a prominent feature relevant to ASD.

Availability of supporting data

Recordings of ultrasonic vocalizations will be made available using mouseTube, an online platform for sharing ultrasonic vocalization recordings that is currently established by Thomas Bourgeron, Elodie Ey, and Nicolas Torquet from Institut Pasteur, Paris, France.

Abbreviations

5,7-DHT:

5,7 dihydroxytryptamine

5-HT:

5-hydroxytryptamine, serotonin

5-HTP:

5-hydroxytryptophan

ASD:

autism spectrum disorders

CNS:

central nervous system

PCPA:

para-chlorophenylalanine

PND:

postnatal day

SERT:

serotonin transporter

SSRI:

selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors

TPH:

tryptophan hydroxylase

USV:

ultrasonic vocalizations

References

  1. Descarries L, Riad M, Parent M. Ultrastructure of the serotonin innervation in the mammalian nervous system. In: Müller CP, Jacobs BL, editors. Handbook of the behavioral neurobiology of serotonin. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2010. p. 65–101.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  2. Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L. The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse molecular genetics. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:1002–10012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Whitaker-Azmitia PM. Serotonin and development. In: Müller CP, Jacobs BL, editors. Handbook of the behavioral neurobiology of serotonin. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2010. p. 309–23.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  4. Hohmann CF, Blue ME. The role of serotonin in cortical development: implications for autism spectrum disorders. In: Müller CP, Jacobs BL, editors. Handbook of the behavioral neurobiology of serotonin. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2010. p. 637–65.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  5. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Publishing, Arlington, VA, USA; 2013

  6. Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, Crawley JN. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11:490–502.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Wöhr M, Scattoni ML. Behavioural methods used in rodent models of autism spectrum disorders: current standards and new developments. Behav Brain Res. 2013;251:5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Michetti C, Ricceri L, Scattoni ML. Modeling social communication deficits in mouse models of autism. Autism. 2012;S1:007.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Scattoni ML, Crawley J, Ricceri L. Ultrasonic vocalizations: a tool for behavioural phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33:508–15.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Wöhr M. Ultrasonic vocalizations in Shank mouse models for autism spectrum disorders: detailed spectrographic analyses and developmental profiles. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;43:199–212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Hård E, Engel J, Musi B. The ontogeny of defensive reactions in the rat: influence of the monoamine transmission systems. Scand J Psychol. 1982;S1:90–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Winslow JT, Insel TR. Serotonergic modulation of rat pup ultrasonic vocal development: studies with 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1990;254:212–20.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Richardson-Jones JW, Craige CP, Nguyen TH, Kung HF, Gardier AM, Dranovsky A, et al. Serotonin-1A autoreceptors are necessary and sufficient for the normal formation of circuits underlying innate anxiety. J Neurosci. 2011;31:6008–18.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Fish EW, Sekinda M, Ferrari PF, Dirks A, Miczek KA. Distress vocalizations in maternally separated mouse pups: modulation via 5-HT(1A), 5-HT(1B) and GABA(A) receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2000;149:277–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Nastiti K, Benton D, Brain PF, Haug M. The effects of 5-HT receptor ligands on ultrasonic calling in mouse pups. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1991;15:483–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Hård E, Engel J. Effects of 8-OH-DPAT on ultrasonic vocalization and audiogenic immobility reaction in pre-weanling rats. Neuropharmacology. 1988;27:981–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Johansson-Wallsten CE, Berg M, Meyerson BJ. The effects of long-term treatment with the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT and the 5-HT2/1C receptor agonist DOI in the neonatal rat. Eur J Pharmacol. 1993;243:149–54.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Joyce MP, Carden SE. The effects of 8-OH-DPAT and (+/-)-pindolol on isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations in 3-, 10-, and 14-day-old rats. Dev Psychobiol. 1999;34:109–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Klint T, Andersson G. Ultrasound vocalization is not related to corticosterone response in isolated rat pups. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;47:947–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Mos J, Olivier B. Ultrasonic vocalizations by rat pups as an animal model for anxiolytic activity: effects of serotonergic drugs. In: Bevan P, Cools AR, Archer T, editors. Behavioural Pharmacology of 5-HT. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1989. p. 361–6.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Olivier B, Molewijk HE, van der Heyden JA, van Oorschot R, Ronken E, Mos J, et al. Ultrasonic vocalizations in rat pups: effects of serotonergic ligands. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1998;23:215–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Shayit M, Yadid G, Overstreet DH, Weller A. 5-HT(1A) receptor subsensitivity in infancy and supersensitivity in adulthood in an animal model of depression. Brain Res. 2003;980:100–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Winslow JT, Insel TR. Serotonergic modulation of the rat pup ultrasonic isolation call: studies with 5HT1 and 5HT2 subtype-selective agonists and antagonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1991;105:513–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Fish EW, Faccidomo S, Gupta S, Miczek KA. Anxiolytic-like effects of escitalopram, citalopram, and R-citalopram in maternally separated mouse pups. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2004;308:474–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Hodgson RA, Guthrie DH, Varty GB. Duration of ultrasonic vocalizations in the isolated rat pup as a behavioral measure: sensitivity to anxiolytic and antidepressant drugs. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008;88:341–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Iijima M, Chaki S. Separation-induced ultrasonic vocalization in rat pups: further pharmacological characterization. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2005;82:652–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Kehne JH, Coverdale S, McCloskey TC, Hoffman DC, Cassella JV. Effects of the CRF(1) receptor antagonist, CP 154,526, in the separation-induced vocalization anxiolytic test in rat pups. Neuropharmacology. 2000;39:1357–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Starr KR, Price GW, Watson JM, Atkinson PJ, Arban R, Melotto S, et al. SB-649915-B, a novel 5-HT1A/B autoreceptor antagonist and serotonin reuptake inhibitor, is anxiolytic and displays fast onset activity in the rat high light social interaction test. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32:2163–72.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Winslow JT, Insel TR. Serotonergic and catecholaminergic reuptake inhibitors have opposite effects on the ultrasonic isolation calls of rat pups. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1990;3:51–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Walther DJ, Peter JU, Bashammakh S, Hörtnagl H, Voits M, Fink H, et al. Synthesis of serotonin by a second tryptophan hydroxylase isoform. Science. 2003;299:76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Alenina N, Kikic D, Todiras M, Mosienko V, Qadri F, Plehm R, et al. Growth retardation and altered autonomic control in mice lacking brain serotonin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:10332–7.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gutknecht L, Araragi N, Merker S, Waider J, Sommerlandt FM, Mlinar B, et al. Impacts of brain serotonin deficiency following Tph2 inactivation on development and raphe neuron serotonergic specification. PLoS One. 2012;7:e43157.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Kane MJ, Angoa-Peréz M, Briggs DI, Sykes CE, Francescutti DM, Rosenberg DR, et al. Mice genetically depleted of brain serotonin display social impairments, communication deficits and repetitive behaviors: possible relevance to autism. PLoS One. 2012;7:e48975.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Migliarini S, Pacini G, Pelosi B, Lunardi G, Pasqualetti M. Lack of brain serotonin affects postnatal development and serotonergic neuronal circuitry formation. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:1106–18.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Narboux-Nême N, Angenard G, Mosienko V, Klempin F, Pitychoutis PM, Deneris E, et al. Postnatal growth defects in mice with constitutive depletion of central serotonin. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2013;4:171–81.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Angoa-Pérez M, Kane MJ, Briggs DI, Sykes CE, Shah MM, Francescutti DM, et al. Genetic depletion of brain 5HT reveals a common molecular pathway mediating compulsivity and impulsivity. J Neurochem. 2012;121:974–84.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Mosienko V, Bert B, Beis D, Matthes S, Fink H, Bader M, et al. Exaggerated aggression and decreased anxiety in mice deficient in brain serotonin. Transl Psychiatry. 2012;2:e122.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Angoa-Pérez M, Kane MJ, Sykes CE, Perrine SA, Church MW, Kuhn DM. Brain serotonin determines maternal behavior and offspring survival. Genes Brain Behav. 2014;13:579–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Mosienko V, Beis D, Pasqualetti M, Waider J, Matthes S, Qadri F, et al. Life without brain serotonin: reevaluation of serotonin function with mice deficient in brain serotonin synthesis. Behav Brain Res. 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.06.005.

  40. Liu Y, Jiang Y, Si Y, Kim JY, Chen ZF, Rao Y. Molecular regulation of sexual preference revealed by genetic studies of 5-HT in the brains of male mice. Nature. 2011;472:95–9.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Zhang S, Liu Y, Rao Y. Serotonin signaling in the brain of adult female mice is required for sexual preference. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:9968–73.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Wöhr M, Roullet FI, Hung AY, Sheng M, Crawley JN. Communication impairments in mice lacking Shank1: reduced levels of ultrasonic vocalizations and scent marking behavior. PLoS One. 2011;6:e20631.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Yang M, Bozdagi O, Scattoni ML, Wöhr M, Roullet FI, Katz AM, et al. Reduced excitatory neurotransmission and mild autism-relevant phenotypes in adolescent Shank3 null mutant mice. J Neurosci. 2012;32:6525–41.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Wöhr M, Dahlhoff M, Wolf E, Holsboer F, Schwarting RKW, Wotjak CT. Effects of genetic background, gender, and early environmental factors on isolation-induced ultrasonic calling in mouse pups: an embryo-transfer study. Behav Genet. 2008;38:579–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Cazalets JR, Gardette M, Hilaire G. Locomotor network maturation is transiently delayed in the MAOA-deficient mouse. J Neurophysiol. 2000;83:2468–70.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ristine LA, Spear LP. Effects of serotonergic and cholinergic antagonists on suckling behavior of neonatal, infant, and weanling rat pups. Behav Neural Biol. 1984;41:99–126.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Hung AY, Futai K, Sala C, Valtschanoff JG, Ryu J, Woodworth MA, et al. Smaller dendritic spines, weaker synaptic transmission, but enhanced spatial learning in mice lacking Shank1. J Neurosci. 2008;28:1697–708.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Silverman JL, Turner SM, Barkan CL, Tolu SS, Saxena R, Hung AY, et al. Sociability and motor functions in Shank1 mutant mice. Brain Res. 2011;1380:120–37.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Sungur AÖ, Vörckel KJ, Schwarting RKW, Wöhr M. Repetitive behaviors in the Shank1 knockout mouse model for autism spectrum disorder: developmental aspects and effects of social context. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;234:92–100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Ey E, Torquet N, Le Sourd AM, Leblond CS, Boeckers TM, Faure P, et al. The Autism ProSAP1/Shank2 mouse model displays quantitative and structural abnormalities in ultrasonic vocalisations. Behav Brain Res. 2013;256:677–89.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hahn ME, Hewitt JK, Schanz N, Weinreb L, Henry A. Genetic and developmental influences on infant mouse ultrasonic calling. I. A diallel analysis of the calls of 3-day olds. Behav Genet. 1997;27:133–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Hahn ME, Benno RH, Schanz N, Phadia E. The effects of prenatal cocaine exposure and genotype on the ultrasonic calls of infant mice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;67:729–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Hahn ME, Schanz N. The effects of cold, rotation, and genotype on the production of ultrasonic calls in infant mice. Behav Genet. 2002;32:267–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Roubertoux PL, Martin B, LeRoy I, Beau J, Marchaland C, Perez-Diaz F, et al. Vocalizations in newborn mice: genetic analysis. Behav Genet. 1996;26:427–37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Hahn ME, Karkowski L, Weinreb L, Henry A, Schanz N. Genetic and developmental influences on infant mouse ultrasonic calling. II. Developmental patterns in the calls of mice 2–12 days of age. Behav Genet. 1998;28:315–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kurz A, Wöhr M, Walter M, Bonin M, Auburger G, Gispert S, et al. Alpha-synuclein deficiency affects brain Foxp1 expression and ultrasonic vocalization. Neuroscience. 2010;17:287–98.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Hofer MA, Shair HN. Ultrasonic vocalization, laryngeal braking, and thermogenesis in rat pups: a reappraisal. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107:354–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Cummings KJ, Li A, Nattie EE. Brainstem serotonin deficiency in the neonatal period: autonomic dysregulation during mild cold stress. J Physiol. 2011;589:2055–64.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Ehret G. Left hemisphere advantage in the mouse brain for recognizing ultrasonic communication calls. Nature. 1987;325:249–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Ehret G. Categorical perception of mouse-pup ultrasounds in the temporal domain. Anim Behav. 1992;43:409–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Ehret G, Haack B. Ultrasound recognition in house mice: key-stimulus configuration and recognition mechanisms. J Comp Physiol. 1982;148:245–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Sewell GD. Ultrasonic communication in rodents. Nature. 1970;227:410.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Smith JC. Responses to adult mice to models infant calls. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1976;90:1105–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Benton D, Nastiti K. The influence of psychotropic drugs on the ultrasonic calling of mouse pups. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1988;95:99–102.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Gardner CR. Distress vocalization in rat pups. A simple screening method for anxiolytic drugs. J Pharmacol Methods. 1985;14:181–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Kehne JH, McCloskey TC, Baron BM, Chi EM, Harrison BL, Whitten JP, et al. NMDA receptor complex antagonists have potential anxiolytic effects as measured with separation-induced ultrasonic vocalizations. Eur J Pharmacol. 1991;193:283–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Podhorna J, Brown RE. Flibanserin has anxiolytic effects without locomotor side effects in the infant rat ultrasonic vocalization model of anxiety. Br J Pharmacol. 2000;130:739–46.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Brunner D, Buhot MC, Hen R, Hofer M. Anxiety, motor activation, and maternal-infant interactions in 5HT1B knockout mice. Behav Neurosci. 1999;113:587–601.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. El-Khodor BF, Dimmler MH, Amara DA, Hofer M, Hen R, Brunner D. Juvenile 5HT(1B) receptor knockout mice exhibit reduced pharmacological sensitivity to 5HT(1A) receptor activation. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2004;22:405–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. van Velzen A, Toth M. Role of maternal 5-HT(1A) receptor in programming offspring emotional and physical development. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9:877–85.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Weller A, Leguisamo AC, Towns L, Ramboz S, Bagiella E, Hofer M, et al. Maternal effects in infant and adult phenotypes of 5HT1A and 5HT1B receptor knockout mice. Dev Psychobiol. 2003;42:194–205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Zanettini C, Carola V, Lo Iacono L, Moles A, Gross C, D’Amato FR. Postnatal handling reverses social anxiety in serotonin receptor 1A knockout mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9:26–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Brunelli SA, Aviles JA, Gannon KS, Branscomb A, Shacham S. PRX-00023, a selective serotonin 1A receptor agonist, reduces ultrasonic vocalizations in infant rats bred for high infantile anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;94:8–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Chugani DC, Muzik O, Behen M, Rothermel R, Janisse JJ, Lee J, et al. Developmental changes in brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autistic and nonautistic children. Ann Neurol. 1999;45:287–95.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Chugani DC, Muzik O, Rothermel R, Behen M, Chakraborty P, Mangner T, et al. Altered serotonin synthesis in the dentatothalamocortical pathway in autistic boys. Ann Neurol. 1997;42:666–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Chandana SR, Behen ME, Juhász C, Muzik O, Rothermel RD, Mangner TJ, et al. Significance of abnormalities in developmental trajectory and asymmetry of cortical serotonin synthesis in autism. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23:171–82.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Makkonen I, Riikonen R, Kokki H, Airaksinen MM, Kuikka JT. Serotonin and dopamine transporter binding in children with autism determined by SPECT. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008;50:593–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Coon H, Dunn D, Lainhart J, Miller J, Hamil C, Battaglia A, et al. Possible association between autism and variants in the brain-expressed tryptophan hydroxylase gene (TPH2). Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005;135B:42–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Voineagu I, Wang X, Johnston P, Lowe JK, Tian Y, Horvath S, et al. Transcriptomic analysis of autistic brain reveals convergent molecular pathology. Nature. 2011;474:380–4.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Yang SY, Yoo HJ, Cho IH, Park M, Kim SA. Association with tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene polymorphisms and autism spectrum disorders in Korean families. Neurosci Res. 2012;73:333–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Delorme R, Durand CM, Betancur C, Wagner M, Ruhrmann S, Grabe HJ, et al. No human tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene R441H mutation in a large cohort of psychiatric patients and control subjects. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:202–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Egawa J, Watanabe Y, Nunokawa A, Endo T, Kaneko N, Tamura R, et al. A detailed association analysis between the tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2) gene and autism spectrum disorders in a Japanese population. Psychiatry Res. 2012;196:320–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Ramoz N, Cai G, Reichert JG, Corwin TE, Kryzak LA, Smith CJ, et al. Family-based association study of TPH1 and TPH2 polymorphisms in autism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006;141B:861–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Sacco R, Papaleo V, Hager J, Rousseau F, Moessner R, Militerni R, et al. Case-control and family-based association studies of candidate genes in autistic disorder and its endophenotypes: TPH2 and GLO1. BMC Med Genet. 2007;8:11.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Veenstra-VanderWeele J, Muller CL, Iwamoto H, Sauer JE, Owens WA, Shah CR, et al. Autism gene variant causes hyperserotonemia, serotonin receptor hypersensitivity, social impairment and repetitive behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:5469–74.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Kerr TM, Muller CL, Miah M, Jetter CS, Pfeiffer R, Shah C, et al. Genetic background modulates phenotypes of serotonin transporter Ala56 knock-in mice. Mol Autism. 2013;4:35.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Jones KL, Smith RM, Edwards KS, Givens B, Tilley MR, Beversdorf DQ. Combined effect of maternal serotonin transporter genotype and prenatal stress in modulating offspring social interaction in mice. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2010;28:529–36.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Dougherty JD, Maloney SE, Wozniak DF, Rieger MA, Sonnenblick L, Coppola G, et al. The disruption of Celf6, a gene identified by translational profiling of serotonergic neurons, results in autism-related behaviors. J Neurosci. 2013;33:2732–53.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Nakatani J, Tamada K, Hatanaka F, Ise S, Ohta H, Inoue K, et al. Abnormal behavior in a chromosome-engineered mouse model for human 15q11-13 duplication seen in autism. Cell. 2009;137:1235–46.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Susanne da Costa Goncalves, Sabine Grüger, and Manfred Ströhmann for the excellent technical assistance, and Catherine Schweppe and Zachary Tasker Hull for their help in data analysis.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) to MW (DFG WO 1732/1-1) and NA (DFG AL 2474/2-1), a grant from the Russian Science Foundation to NA (â„– 14-50-00069), and a fellowship by the German Academic Exchange Service (Deutscher Akademischer Austauschdienst) to VM (DAAD A07/99669).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Markus Wöhr.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MW designed the research; VM and DB performed the research; VM, DB, and MW analyzed the data; VM, DB, NA, and MW wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mosienko, V., Beis, D., Alenina, N. et al. Reduced isolation-induced pup ultrasonic communication in mouse pups lacking brain serotonin. Molecular Autism 6, 13 (2015). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13229-015-0003-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13229-015-0003-6

Keywords